metricas
covid
Buscar en
Revista de Psicodidáctica (English Edition)
Toda la web
Inicio Revista de Psicodidáctica (English Edition) Using profile analysis and ROC curves to examine the relationship between perfec...
Journal Information
Visits
7
Original
Full text access
Available online 18 February 2024
Using profile analysis and ROC curves to examine the relationship between perfectionism and academic self-efficacy in secondary school students
Empleando análisis de perfiles y curvas ROC para examinar la relación entre el perfeccionismo y la autoeficacia académica en estudiantes de Educación Secundaria
Visits
7
Andrea Fuster-Ricoa, María Vicenta,
Corresponding author
maria.vicent@ua.es

Corresponding author.
, Carolina Gonzálveza, María Pérez-Marcoa, Lucía Granados-Alósb, Raquel Suriác
a Department of Developmental Psychology and Teaching, University of Alicante, Spain
b Faculty of Education, International University of Valencia, Spain
c Department of Communication and Social Psychology, University of Alicante, Spain
This item has received
Received 01 October 2023. Accepted 11 January 2024
Article information
Abstract
Full Text
Bibliography
Download PDF
Statistics
Figures (2)
Tables (5)
Table 1. Sample distribution by sex and age
Table 2. Indices of fit for the results of the latent profile analysis
Table 3. Means and standard deviations of each latent profile obtained on academic self-efficacy
Table 4. Pearson correlation indices and Cohen's d indices for post hoc contrasts between the mean scores obtained by the four profiles on academic self-efficacy
Table 5. Binary logistic regression for the probability of receiving high scores on self-efficacy as a function of the perfectionism variables
Show moreShow less
Abstract

This study attempts to clarify the relationship between multidimensional perfectionism and academic self-efficacy in adolescents using a dual approach: variable-oriented and person-oriented. The study sample consisted of 1375 students aged 15 to 18 (M = 16.36, SD = 1.04). The Child and Adolescent Perfectionism Scale and the Perceived Self-Efficacy in Academic Situations Scale were used. Four perfectionist profiles were obtained from a combination of socially prescribed perfectionism (SPP) and self-oriented perfectionism (SOP) based on the Latent Profile Analysis: very low perfectionism, low perfectionism, high perfectionism, and moderate perfectionism. The high perfectionism group scored significantly higher on academic self-efficacy than the other groups. Furthermore, post-hoc comparisons revealed statistically significant differences in academic self-efficacy between all profiles, with moderate to large effect sizes, with the exception of the very low and low perfectionism groups. Logistic regressions demonstrated that SOP and SPP positively and significantly predicted high scores on academic self-efficacy. Upon analysis of the ROC curves, it was found that both SOP and SPP display good and similar discriminative ability, correctly classifying 79% and 76% of the participants with and without high academic self-efficacy levels, respectively. Possible explanations and implications for Educational Psychology are discussed.

Keywords:
Perfectionism
Academic self-efficacy
Adolescent population
Latent profile analysis
ROC curves
Resumen

El presente estudio pretende contribuir a esclarecer la relación que existe entre el perfeccionismo multidimensional y la autoeficacia académica en población juvenil a partir de un doble enfoque: centrado en la persona y en la variable. La muestra está compuesta de 1.375 estudiantes de entre 15 y 18 años (M = 16.36, DT = 1.04). Se emplean la Child and Adolescent Perfectionism Scale y la Escala de Autoeficacia Percibida en Situaciones Académicas. A través del análisis de perfiles latentes se obtienen cuatro perfiles perfeccionistas resultado de la interacción entre el perfeccionismo socialmente prescrito (PSP) y el perfeccionismo autoorientado (PAO): perfeccionismo muy bajo, perfeccionismo bajo, perfeccionismo alto y perfeccionismo moderado. El grupo con perfeccionismo alto puntúa positiva y significativamente más alto en autoeficacia académica que el resto. Además, las comparaciones post-hoc muestran que, en relación con la autoeficacia académica, existen diferencias estadísticamente significativas entre todos los perfiles con tamaños del efecto moderados y grandes, excepto para aquellos con perfeccionismo muy bajo y perfeccionismo bajo. Las regresiones logísticas evidencian que el PAO y el PSP predicen de forma positiva y significativa altas puntuaciones en autoeficacia académica. Al emplear el análisis de las Curvas ROC, se obtiene que la capacidad discriminativa tanto del PAO como del PSP es buena y similar, clasificando correctamente al 79% y 76% de los participantes con y sin altos niveles de autoeficacia académica. Se discuten las posibles explicaciones y las implicaciones de los resultados para el ámbito de la Psicología de la Educación.

Palabras clave:
Perfeccionismo
Autoeficacia académica
Población juvenil
Análisis de perfiles latentes
Curvas ROC
Full Text
Introduction

The study of perfectionism, a complex and multidimensional personality trait (Flett et al., 2022), is increasingly frequent, with regard to both its origins and its development (Jeong & Ryan, 2022; Vicent et al., 2017). This trait is notable given its close link to psychopathology in general (Limburg et al., 2017) and its high prevalence in clinical and community populations, with rates that have increased exponentially over recent decades (Curran & Hill, 2019). Although no consensus exists regarding the definition of perfectionism, Flett et al. (2016) attributed two dimensions to child and youth perfectionism. On the one hand, socially prescribed perfectionism (SPP) refers to the belief that others demand that an individual achieve perfection, while, on the other hand, self-oriented perfectionism (SOP) refers to self-criticism and striving for perfection, as well as the self-imposition of high standards. Although diverse studies have positively linked certain perfectionism dimensions with adjustment variables such as academic performance (Madigan, 2019), psychological well-being (Kamushadze et al., 2021), conscientiousness (Di Fabio et al., 2019), and general and creative self-efficacy (Goulet-Pelletier et al., 2022); others have clearly maladaptive effects (Chemisquy et al., 2019). Therefore, although perfectionism may be viewed from a positive perspective (Kinman & Grant, 2022), considerable pressure to be perfect exists amongst adolescents, causing a major social problem (Campeau et al., 2023).

Perfectionism and academic self-efficacy

Academic self-efficacy has been defined as an individual’s set of beliefs regarding his/her ability to achieve a desired result through academic tasks in distinct settings, circumstances, and difficulty levels (Ford et al., 2023; Wuthrich et al., 2021). In the educational setting, it is a variable of great relevance given its impact on student motivation and learning (Codella et al., 2020). Therefore, it has been positively and significantly related to academic performance (Martínez et al., 2021; Weber & Harzer, 2022; Zeinalipour, 2022), emotional implication (Bostan et al., 2022), and school commitment and belonging (Yang et al., 2022). In addition, it has been found to develop in large part from how students perceive and interpret major sources of information (Ford et al., 2023). This is the area in which perfectionism may play a relevant role. High self-imposed standards or demands of others and a predisposition to seek perfection may influence the student’s academic self-efficacy development (Ford et al., 2023).

Few studies have provided preliminary data on the relationship between perfectionism and academic self-efficacy in children and adolescents. One study published by Bong et al. (2014), using a sample of 304 students aged 12 and 13, found that SOP is positively and significantly correlated with academic self-efficacy in the areas of mathematics and English language, but this was not the case with SPP. Along these lines, Ford et al. (2023) examined the relationship between perfectionism and mathematical academic self-efficacy in a sample of 1,683 adolescents aged 11 to 14, finding that mathematical self-efficacy is positively and significantly related to SOP, while results were not significant for SPP.

The other scientific findings with regard to the link between these two variables focuses on the analysis of general self-efficacy in university students or adults, having contradicting results (Arazzini-Stewart & De George-Walker, 2014; Kurtovic et al.,2019; Pázozdy et al., 2023; Seo, 2008; Wang et al., 2023; Willians & Edwards, 2022). On the one hand, Seo (2008), Wang et al. (2023) and Willians and Edwards (2022) found a positive and statistically significant relationship between SOP and general self-efficacy. However, Wang et al. (2023) observed that SPP was significantly and negatively linked to self-efficacy. On the other hand, using other indicators of perfectionism, distinct from SOP and SPP, Arazzini-Stewart and De George-Walker (2014), reported that there is a significant and negative relationship existing between perfectionism and self-efficacy. Kurtovic et al. (2019) found that self-efficacy had a statistically significant negative relationship with discrepancy, while for standards and order, there was a positive and statistically significant relationship. Finally, Pázozdy et al. (2023) also analyzed the bivariate correlations, which revealed negative associations between perfectionism and self-efficacy, only reaching statistical significance in the female samples.

After reviewing the literature on perfectionism and academic self-efficacy, a series of limitations were found. Firstly, only two studies specifically focused on academic self-efficacy, having results that coincided in finding positive and statistically significant relationships between mathematical academic self-efficacy and SOP, with this not being the case for SPP (Bong et al., 2014; Ford et al., 2023). However, these results only provide data regarding self-efficacy within this specific area of knowledge, so they cannot be extrapolated to overall academic self-efficacy. Secondly, although childhood and adolescence are sensitive periods for the development of perfectionism (Damian et al., 2022), all of the studies were carried out on university student populations, except for two that considered a child/youth population (Bong et al., 2014; Ford et al., 2023) and the study by Willians and Edwards (2022) considering an adult population.

Finally, the cited works took a variable-oriented approach as opposed to a person-oriented one. Currently, however, the person-centered approach is considered an emerging research line in the field of perfectionism (e.g. Haraldsen et al., 2021; Seong & Chang, 2021; Stornaes et al., 2019; Vicent et al., 2017; Vicent, Inglés, Gonzálvez, Sanmartín, Aparicio-Flores, et al., 2019; Vicent, Inglés, Gonzálvez, Sanmartín, Ortega-Sandoval, et al., 2019; Vicent et al., 2021; Vicent et al., 2022), since it permits the identification of how perfectionist dimensions combine through profiles, presenting the results in terms of the adaptation and mismatch obtained by each profile, to better reflect the reality experienced by individuals (Vicent, Inglés, Gonzálvez, Sanmartín, Aparicio-Flores, et al., 2019). Very few studies on perfectionist profiles have been conducted with child and adolescent populations. Furthermore, the profile solutions obtained tend to vary from study to study, perhaps due to the use of different scales to measure perfectionism. For example, Vicent, Inglés, Gonzálvez, Sanmartín, Aparicio-Flores, et al. (2019) and Vicent et al. (2022) used the Child and Adolescent Perfectionism Scale (CAPS; Flett et al., 2016) and obtained three perfectionism profiles (high perfectionism, moderate perfectionism, and no perfectionism) whereas Haraldsen et al. (2021) used the Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (FMPS; Frost et al., 1990) and identified four profiles (no perfectionism, perfectionism dominated by effort, perfectionism dominated by concerns, and mixed perfectionism).

The present study

This study attempts to clarify the relationship between multidimensional perfectionism and academic self-efficacy in adolescents using a dual approach: person-oriented and variable-oriented. First, (1) it attempted to identify profiles of youth perfectionism resulting from the combination of the SPP and SOP dimensions. Next, (2) it aimed to verify whether the identified profiles varied from one another in terms of academic self-efficacy levels. Based on past literature that identified classes of perfectionism using the CAPS, three classes were expected to be found, consisting of high, moderate, and no perfectionism (Vicent, Inglés, Gonzálvez, Sanmartín, Aparicio-Flores, et al., 2019; Vicent et al., 2022). Assuming that this three-class model is found, it is expected that the high perfectionism in SOP group will have the highest levels of academic self-efficacy since, according to Bong et al. (2014) and Ford et al. (2023), there is a significant positive association between SOP and mathematics academic self-efficacy, but this is not the case for SPP. Second, using a variable-oriented approach, (3) the study analyzes the predictive capacity of attaining high scores on academic self-efficacy according to both perfectionist dimensions, SOP, and SPP; (4) and the discriminative capacity of both subscales for identifying individuals with and without high levels of academic self-efficacy. In this way, according to Bong et al. (2014) and Ford et al. (2023), it was expected that the SOP dimension would have a positive predictive capacity and a good discriminative capacity regarding the condition of having or not having high self-efficacy.

MethodParticipants

Study participants were selected by randomized cluster sampling. The primary geographic area was the province of Alicante: center, north, south, east, and west. The secondary units are the compulsory secondary education institutes (two to three, selected randomly and in proportion in each area, choosing 15 public and private institutes). Classrooms were the tertiary units, with four being randomly selected, one per course year from the 3rd year of compulsory secondary education to the 2nd year of baccalaureate studies. Following this system, the sample consisted of 1,375 students aged 15 to 18 (M = 16.36, SD = 1.04), of which 695 were boys and 680 were girls. Table 1 shows the distribution of the sample based on sex and age. The sample is homogenous, as revealed by the Chi-squared test (χ² = 3.103, p =  .38). The ethnic composition of the sample is: 86.34% Spanish, 6.79% Latin American, 4.12% Arab, 2.55% other European, and 0.20% were Asian. In most cases, the socioeconomic level of the participants was average.

Table 1.

Sample distribution by sex and age

  15 years old  16 years old  17 years old  18 years old  Total 
Boys  16912.3%  21615.7%  19214.0%  1188.6%  69550.5% 
Girls  18913.7%  18713.6%  18913.7%  1158.4%  68049.5% 
Total  35826.0%  40329.3%  38127.7%  23316.9%  1375100% 
Instruments

Child and Adolescent Perfectionism Scale (CAPS; Flett et al., 2016). This scale evaluated SPP (ten items; e.g. «I want to be better in everything that I do») and SOP (12 items; e.g. «I feel like people ask too much about me») using a 5-point Likert-like scale (1 = false, 5 = very true). Reliability levels for SOP in this study are α = .80 and Ω = .81, whereas for SPP they are α = .80 and Ω = .82. The scale has been validated in Spain for a population of children aged 8 to 12 (Vicent, Inglés, Sanmartín, et al., 2019).

Scale of Perceived Self-efficacy in Academic Situations (EAPESA; García-Fernández et al., 2010). This instrument has the objective of measuring the expectations of children and university students in terms of their self-efficacy in educational situations. It consists of 10 items (e.g. «I consider myself to be sufficiently capable of successfully taking on any academic task») measured using a 4-point Likert-like scale (1 = never, 4 = always), having a reported reliability for this study of α = .85 and Ω = .87.

Procedure

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Alicante (UA-2023-03-07). A meeting was held with the directors of the selected schools to inform them of the proposed objectives, inviting them to collaborate in this project. In addition, written parental consent was requested. The instruments were applied collectively and anonymously during school hours. The average time for instrument administration was 15 minutes for the CAPS and five minutes for the EAPESA.

Statistical analysis

Bivariate correlations between the perfectionist dimensions and the ten items making up the EAPESA were analyzed. According to the values proposed by Cohen (1988), the magnitude of these correlations was considered small when the values range between 0.10 and 0.29; moderate between 0.30 and 0.49, and large for values equal to or greater than 0.50. For profile identification, a latent profile analysis was performed. The choice of the optimal profile was obtained from the theoretical interpretability of each model and considering the indices proposed by Song and Kim (2019): the lowest values of the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) and the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC); p values under .05 for the Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood-Ratio Test (LRT) and the Bootstrap Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT); and entropy scores approaching 1. In addition to these indices and statistics, to add sense to the classification by classes, no solution was considered if it included small profiles (with less than 25 subjects). These analyses were carried out using the MPLUS 8.10 program.

After determining the best fit of the models of perfectionism profiles, interclass differences in the mean scores of the 10 EAPESA items were calculated using analysis of variance (ANOVA). Post hoc tests (Bonferroni method) were also performed to identify where there were statistically significant differences between the perfectionism profiles with regard to academic self-efficacy. In addition, the effect size was obtained through calculation of Cohen (1988) to determine the size of the differences found, interpreted as follows: d values between 0.20 and 0.49, between 0.50 and 0.79, and above 0.80 for small, moderate, and large effect sizes, respectively.

Binary logistic regression was used to analyze the predictive capacity of SOP and SPP on high scores on academic self-efficacy (scores above or equal to the 75th centile) using the forward stepwise regression procedure based on the Wald statistic. The predictive capacity was estimated using the Odds Ratio (OR), with values greater than 1 being considered a positive prediction; values less than 1 being negative predictions; and values equaling one being an absence of predictive capacity (De Maris, 2003). Both analyses of variance and logistic regressions are performed using the SPSS 28 program.

Finally, in order to verify the discriminative capacity of the scores on perfectionism to identify subjects with or without high levels of academic self-efficacy, the ROC curves were calculated (sensitivity versus specificity) separately for each perfectionism dimension (e.g., SOP and SPP). To interpret the value of the area under the curve (AUC), the following criteria were considered: between .75 and .90, it was estimated that the test is good; between .90 and .97, it is assessed that the discriminative capacity is very good; values between .97 and 1 indicate an excellent discrimination (Martínez-Pérez & Pérez-Martín, 2023). Sensitivity, or the true positive ratio, is operationalized as the percentage of subjects with high self-efficacy who are correctly classified using the score obtained on each of the perfectionism dimensions (SOP and SPP). Specificity, or the true negative ratio, is operationalized as the percentage of subjects that do not present high self-efficacy and who are identified using the score on each of the perfectionism dimensions. To determine the cut-off point determining the highest sensitivity and specificity, the Youden index was used. In this case, the MedCalc 19 program was used.

ResultsCorrelations between the perfectionist dimensions and academic self-efficacy

Positive and statistically significant bilateral correlations of a moderate nature were observed between both perfectionist dimensions and academic self-efficacy (SOP: r = .41, p <  .001; SPP: r = .34, p <  .001).

Analysis of latent profiles of perfectionism

Table 2 presents the fit obtained for each model from two to seven profiles. The six profile model obtained the lowest AIC and BIC, and a p <  .001 for the BLRT. However, this is rejected since, in addition to not having the highest entropy, it has two clusters with less than 25 subjects. Therefore, the models with five and seven profiles were rejected, even though both had low data in terms of AIC, BIC, and adjusted BIC and a p <  .001 for the BLRT. Models of two and three profiles were also rejected since they had the highest BIC levels, although p <  .001 for the LRT and BLRT. As for the four profile model, it had low values of AIC and BIC, as well as the highest entropy since it was the closest to 1 and a p < .001 for BLRT, and all of the groups are representative of the sample. Therefore, the four profile model was selected since it had a greater classificatory utility and interpretability and given its adequate levels on all of the examined indices of fit.

Table 2.

Indices of fit for the results of the latent profile analysis

Models  AIC  BIC  Adjusted BIC  LRT  Adjusted LRT  BLRT  Entropy  Size 
7307.92  7344.50  7322.27  <.001  <.001  <.001  .68 
6972.06  7024.32  6992.55  <.001  <.001  <.001  .75 
4  6871.15  6939.09  6897.79  .002  .003  <.001  .77  0 
6819.05  6902.67  6851.85  .099  .106  <.001  .72 
6801.68  6900.98  6840.62  .050  .055  <.001  .75 
6790.35  6905.32  6835.44  .165  .176  <.001  .74 

Note. AIC = Akaike Information Criteria; BIC = Bayesian Information Criteria; LRT = Vuong-Lo- Mendell-Rubin Likelihood-Ratio Test; BLRT = Bootstrap Likelihood Ratio Test; Bold data = selected model.

The four profile model consists of four groups of perfectionism. The first classifies 35 (2.5%) students with very low mean scores, both on SOP (-2.61) and on SPP (-2.44), therefore, they are categorized as students with very low perfectionism levels. The second consists of 244 (17.7%) students and they had low scores for the SOP (-1.14) and SPP (-1.19), therefore, they are called students with low perfectionism levels. The third group includes 354 (25.7%) participants with high scores on SOP (1) and SPP (1.07), considered to have high perfectionism levels. Finally, the fourth profile is made up of 742 (54 %) adolescents receiving moderate scores on SOP (−0.05) and SPP (−0.06), therefore, this profile is labelled as moderate perfectionism (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Graphic representation of perfectionism profiles obtained from the latent profile analysis.

(0.06MB).

The results of the ANOVA reveal statistically significant differences between the average scores on academic self-efficacy obtained by each perfectionist profile F(3, 1371)= 72.46, p = < .001, η2 = .14. The high perfectionism profile had the highest means in terms of academic self-efficacy (M = 29.31, SD = 5.40), while the very low perfectionism profile had the lowest mean (M = 20.80, SD = 7.44) (Table 3).

Table 3.

Means and standard deviations of each latent profile obtained on academic self-efficacy

Profiles  Very low perfectionismLow perfectionismHigh perfectionismModerate perfectionismStatistical significance
  M  SD  M  SD  M  DS  M  SD  F(3,1371)  p  η2 
Self-efficacy  20.80  7.33  23.29  5.67  29.21  5.40  26.32  5.13  72.46  <.001  .14 

Post hoc comparisons (Table 4) reveal that, with regard to academic self-efficacy, there are statistically significant differences between all of the profiles except between the very low perfectionism and low perfectionism ones. In addition, the effect sizes associated with these differences are large (d ≥ 0.80) for the comparisons between the very low perfectionism profile and those with high (d = -.1.50) or moderate (d = -1.05) perfectionism, and between the low and high perfectionism (d = -1.07) profiles. On the other hand, comparisons between low and moderate perfectionism (d = 0.58) and between high and moderate perfectionism (d = 0.55) had a moderate effect size.

Table 4.

Pearson correlation indices and Cohen's d indices for post hoc contrasts between the mean scores obtained by the four profiles on academic self-efficacy

ProfilesVery low perfectionism vs. low perfectionism  Very low perfectionism vs. high perfectionism  Very low perfectionism vs. moderate perfectionism  Low perfectionism vs. high perfectionism  Low perfectionism vs. moderate perfectionism  High perfectionism vs. moderate perfectionism 
Self-efficacyp  n. s.  <.001  <.001  <.001  <.001  <.001 
d  –  −1.50  −1.05  −1.07  −0.58  0.55 
Logistic regressions

Table 5 offers the results of the logistic regression analysis for the probability of having high scores on academic self-efficacy based on the perfectionism dimensions. The proportion of correct cases is 72.3% (SOP) and 70.9% (SPP). Furthermore, the R2 values of Nagelkerke ranged between .29 for SOP and .25 for SPP. Both dimensions of the CAPS positively and significantly predicted high scores on academic self-efficacy. Specifically, a level of OR = 1.13 was found for both SOP and SPP. Therefore, the probability of having high academic self-efficacy scores is 13% higher with each point that the SOP and SPP scores increase.

Table 5.

Binary logistic regression for the probability of receiving high scores on self-efficacy as a function of the perfectionism variables

Variable    χ2  R2  B  SE  Wald  p  OR  CI 95% 
SOP  Correctly classified.: 72.3 %  156.79  .29  .12  .01  124.06  <.001  1.13  1.10–1.16 
  Constant      −4.28  .24  101.93  <.001  .01   
SPP  Correctly classified.: 70.9 %  140.09  .25  .12  <.001  107.74  <.001  1.13  1.10–1.16 
  Constant      -3.56  <.001  85.74  <.001  .02   

Note. χ2 = Chi squared; R2 = Nagelkerke squared; B = Regression coefficient; SE = Standard Error; Wald = Wald test; p = Probability; OR = Odds Ratio; CI = Confidence interval at 95%.

ROC curves

Figure 2 represents the area under the curve (AUC), which determines the discriminative capacity of the scores on SOP and SPP to identify subjects with or without high academic self-efficacy. The results for SOP suggest that the AUC value for the cut-off point of 38 is .79 (95% CI = .76–.82), being significant versus chance or a random ROC line (p <  .001); sensitivity is 70.3; specificity is 78.7 and the Youden index is .49 (95% CI = .43–.55) suggesting a good discriminative capacity of the SOP subscale.

Figure 2.

ROC curves for the discriminative capacity of the scores on SOP (left) and SPP (right) over high self-efficacy.

(0.2MB).

Regarding SPP, the AUC value found for the cut-off point of 31 was .76 (95% CI = .72–.79, p < .001), and sensitivity and specificity values of 70.1 and 70.6, respectively suggesting a good discriminative capacity for the cut-off point of .31 (Figure 2), having a Youden Index value of .44 (95% CI = .34–.54).

Discussion

This study aims to clarify whether a relationship exists between multidimensional perfectionism and academic self-efficacy in youth, based on a dual approach: person-oriented and variable-oriented. First, the results of the latent profile analysis permitted observation of the existence of four student profiles reflecting very low, low, high and moderate perfectionism. This model does not coincide with the proposed hypothesis, despite the fact that the CAPS is used as in the studies by Vicent, Inglés, Gonzálvez, Sanmartín, Aparicio-Flores, et al. (2019) and Vicent et al. (2022). However, diverse studies using this procedure have identified 4 perfectionism profiles (Çimşir & Ülker-Tümlü, 2021; Gustafsson et al., 2016; Haraldsen et al., 2021; Seong & Chang, 2021; Vicent et al., 2021). This may mean that the differences between the number of perfectionism profiles identified in the scientific literature is more dependent on specific sample characteristics or researcher interpretation (Çimşir & Ülker-Tümlü, 2021) than on the instruments used to assess this construct.

Regarding the behavior of both perfectionism dimensions for each profile, our results are similar to those obtained by Vicent, Inglés, Gonzálvez, Sanmartín, Aparicio-Flores, et al. (2019) in that no profile was found with distinct intensity levels for SPP and SOP. This may be because, as Greenspon (2014) and Lundh et al. (2008) suggested, the perfectionism facets tend to be manifested jointly and cannot be separated in daily life.

As for the results of the ANOVA, adolescents with a high perfectionism profile for both dimensions reported the highest means in terms of academic self-efficacy. This may be because they act as an ambitious group (Lin & Muenks, 2022) having a desire to be perfect that leads them to maintain high levels of effort, persistence, and dedication, in order to achieve their proposed objectives (Vicent, Inglés, Gonzálvez, Sanmartín, Aparicio-Flores, et al., 2019). Or perhaps, this is the type of students developing a larger number of adaptive traits, reducing their levels of negative self-compassion and imposter’s syndrome (Liu et al., 2023). Furthermore, statistically significant differences are found between all profiles, except for the very low perfectionism and low perfectionism profiles, which achieved moderate and large effect sizes. This clarifies the levels of student academic self-efficacy according to their perfectionism profile, suggesting that the higher the level of perfectionism in both dimensions, SPP and SOP, the greater the levels of self-efficacy.

These results obtained through the person-oriented approach are complemented by those derived from the variable-oriented one. Thus, the results of the logistic regressions reveal that both SOP and SPP positively and significantly predict high scores on academic self-efficacy. Along these lines, although the results of the analysis of the ROC curves cannot be compared with those of other studies, since no previous research has incorporated them in the field of perfectionism and academic self-efficacy, they do reflect that the discriminative capacity of both SOP and SPP is good and similar for both dimensions, permitting the correct classification of 79% and 76% of individuals with and without high levels of academic self-efficacy. The cut-off points that best discriminate subjects with and without high levels of academic self-efficacy are .38 for SOP and .31 for SPP. In both cases, sensitivity and specificity levels above .70 are obtained, indicating that these cut-off points permit the correct classification of over 70% of the subjects with high levels of academic self-efficacy (sensitivity), and without high levels of academic self-efficacy (specificity).

Collectively, the results confirm the proposed hypothesis and partially coincide with the findings of the past correlational studies conducted by Bong et al. (2014) and Ford et al. (2023), who provided preliminary data on the relationship between SOP, SPP, and mathematical academic self-efficacy. Thus, while in these studies only one of the two perfectionist dimensions is significantly linked to academic self-efficacy, the results found in this study offer sufficient evidence to consider that both dimensions, SPP and SOP, may be positively and significantly linked to academic self-efficacy. It is possible that certain perfectionist traits such as high standards, conscientiousness when carrying out tasks, order, or self-criticism may have an impact on the development of academic self-efficacy. It is also worth noting that social expectations and judgments, which take place mainly within the family environment, may enhance academic self-efficacy, since they are associated with the development of both SOP and PPS (Smith et al., 2022).

Limitations and future research lines

This study has certain limitations that must be considered. First, the data have been collected using a self-reporting technique, thus there is the possibility of social desirability biases. Secondly, caution must be taken when generalizing the results to other age groups and cultural contexts distinct from those used in this work. Therefore, it may be interesting to replicate this study in other countries in order to make comparisons and observe whether cultural differences affect how perfectionism is linked to academic self-efficacy. Finally, given the transversal design, it is not possible to make conclusions regarding causality between the considered variables. Therefore, longitudinal data should be included to permit the study of student perfectionist tendencies during distinct educational phases.

Conclusions and practical implications

Although this study has certain limitations, it offers novel contributions to the field of Educational Psychology, given that, on the one hand, it allows us to clarify the relationship between SOP, SPP, and academic self-efficacy using a dual focus (variable-oriented and person-oriented), in addition to being the first international study to include the analysis of ROC curves to understand the discriminative capacity of perfectionist dimensions over having self-efficacy in the educational setting. Psychology and educational professionals should consider that having a high academic self-efficacy may be a typical and adaptive characteristic of highly perfectionist students. However, given the close relationship between perfectionism, especially SPP, and psychopathology, in general (Limburg et al., 2017), we should not forget the potential risks associated with highly perfectionist profiles. Therefore, having high levels of academic self-efficacy and even success in studies is not incompatible with dissatisfaction or other forms of negative emotionality, since this may depend upon the students’ skills to manage them (e.g. responsibility or tenacity) (Postigo et al.,2021; Serrano et al., 2022).

Funding

This study was financed by the Department of Innovation, Universities, Science and Digital Society of the (Spain) Generalitat Valenciana with the projects CIGE/2021/021 and CIAICO/2022/078, granted to MV and CG, respectively, as well as with the pre-doctorate assistance CIACIF/2022/252 granted to AFR.

References
[Arazzini-Stewart and De George-Walker, 2014]
M. Arazzini-Stewart, L. De George-Walker.
Self-handicapping, perfectionism, locus of control and self-efficacy: A path model.
Personality and Individual Differences, 66 (2014), pp. 160-164
[Bong et al., 2014]
M. Bong, A. Hwang, A. Noh, S. Kim.
Perfectionism and motivation of adolescents in academic contexts.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 106 (2014), pp. 711-729
[Bostan et al., 2022]
C.M. Bostan, A.C. Apostol, R.L. Andronic, T. Stanciu, T. Constantin.
Type of goals and perceived control for goal achievement over time. The mediating role of motivational persistence.
Acta Psychologica, 231 (2022), pp. 1-9
[Campeau et al., 2023]
G. Campeau, G. Yale-Soulière, C. Gauthier, L. Turgeon.
Perfectionism in schools.
Canadian Psychology, 64 (2023), pp. 118-131
[Chemisquy et al., 2019]
S. Chemisquy, L. Oros, M. Serppe, C. Ernst.
Caracterización del perfeccionismo disfuncional en la niñez tardía.
Apuntes Universitarios, 9 (2019), pp. 1-26
[Çimşir and Ülker-Tümlü, 2021]
E. Çimşir, G. Ülker-Tümlü.
The roles of latent perfectionism classes in academicians’ tendencies toward workaholism, useless superiority effort and narcissism.
The Journal of General Psychology, 149 (2021), pp. 524-549
[Codella et al., 2020]
R. Codella, M.V. Puci, M. Vandoni, L. Correale, C. Galvani, F. Togni, F. Casolo, A. Passi, C. Orizio, G. Alberti, F. Esposito, C. Montomoli, A. La Torre.
School self-efficacy is affected by gender and motor skills: Findings from an Italian study.
[Cohen, 1988]
J. Cohen.
Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences.
Erlbaum, (1988),
[Curran and Hill, 2019]
T. Curran, A.P. Hill.
Perfectionism is increasing over time: A meta-analysis of birth cohort differences from 1989 to 2016.
Psychological Bulletin, 145 (2019), pp. 410-429
[Damian et al., 2022]
L.E. Damian, O. Negru-Subtirica, E.I. Pop, J. Stoeber.
Becoming a perfectionistic adolescent: Perceived parental behaviors involved in developmental trajectories of perfectionism.
European Journal of Personality, 36 (2022), pp. 24-36
[De Maris, 2003]
A. De Maris.
Logistic regression.
Research methods in psychology, pp. 509-532
[Di Fabio et al., 2019]
A. Di Fabio, M.M. Smith, D.H. Salklofse.
Perfectionism and healthy attitude toward oneself: Could humor be a resource?.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (2019), pp. 1-11
[Flett et al., 2016]
G.L. Flett, P.L. Hewitt, A. Besser, C. Su, T. Vaillancourt, D. Boucher, Y. Munro, L.A. Davidson, O. Gale.
The Child-adolescent Perfectionism Scale: Development, psychometric properties, and associations with stress, distress, and psychiatric symptoms.
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 34 (2016), pp. 634-652
[Flett et al., 2022]
G.L. Flett, P.L. Hewitt, T. Nepon, S.B. Sherry, M. Smith.
The destructiveness and public health significance of socially prescribed perfectionism: A review, analysis, and conceptual extension.
Clinical Psychology Review, 93 (2022), pp. 1-21
[Ford et al., 2023]
C.J. Ford, E.L. Usher, V.L. Scott, X.Y. Chen.
The’ perfect’ lens: Perfectionism and early adolescents’ math self-efficacy development.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 93 (2023), pp. 211-228
[Frost et al., 1990]
R.O. Frost, P. Marten, C.M. Lahart, R. Rosenblate.
The dimensions of perfectionism.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14 (1990), pp. 449-468
[García-Fernández et al., 2010]
J.M. García-Fernández, C.J. Inglés, M.S. Torregrosa, C. Ruiz-Esteban, A. Díaz-Herrero, E. Pérez-Fernández, M.C. Martínez-Monteagudo.
Propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Autoeficacia Percibida Específica de Situaciones Académicas en una muestra de estudiantes españoles de Educación Secundaria Obligatoria.
European Journal of Education and Psychology, 3 (2010), pp. 61-74
[Goulet-Pelletier et al., 2022]
J.G. Goulet-Pelletier, P. Gaudreau, D. Cousineau.
Is perfectionism a killer of creative thinking? A test of the model of excellencism and perfectionism.
British Journal of Psychology, 113 (2022), pp. 176-207
[Greenspon, 2014]
T.S. Greenspon.
Is there an antidote to perfectionism?.
Psychology in the Schools, 51 (2014), pp. 986-998
[Gustafsson et al., 2016]
H. Gustafsson, A.P. Hill, A. Stenling, S. Wagnsson.
Profiles of perfectionism, parental climate, and burnout among competitive junior athletes.
Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 26 (2016), pp. 1256-1264
[Haraldsen et al., 2021]
H.M. Haraldsen, A. Ivarsson, B.E. Solstad, F.E. Abrahamsen, H. Halvari.
Composites of perfectionism and inauthenticity in relation to controlled motivation, performance anxiety and exhaustion among elite junior performers.
European Journal of Sport Science, 21 (2021), pp. 428-438
[Jeong and Ryan, 2022]
S.S.Y. Jeong, C. Ryan.
A critical review of child perfectionism as it relates to music pedagogy.
Psychology of Music, 50 (2022), pp. 1312-1337
[Kamushadze et al., 2021]
T. Kamushadze, K. Martskvishvili, M. Mestvirishvili, M. Odilavadze.
Does perfectionism lead to well-being? The role of flow and personality traits.
Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 17 (2021), pp. 43-57
[Kinman and Grant, 2022]
G. Kinman, L. Grant.
Being ‘good enough’: Perfectionism and well-being in social workers.
British Journal of Social Work, 52 (2022), pp. 4171-4188
[Kurtovic et al., 2019]
A. Kurtovic, G. Vrdoljak, A. Idzanovic.
Predicting procrastination: The role of academic achievement, self-efficacy, and perfectionism..
International Journal of Educational Psychology, 8 (2019), pp. 1-26
[Limburg et al., 2017]
K. Limburg, H.J. Watson, M.S. Hagger, S.J. Egan.
The relationship between perfectionism and psychopathology: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 73 (2017), pp. 1301-1326
[Lin and Muenks, 2022]
S. Lin, K. Muenks.
Perfectionism profiles among college students: A person-centered approach to motivation, behavior, and emotion.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 71 (2022), pp. 1-15
[Liu et al., 2023]
L.M. Liu, Y.C. Han, Z. Lu, C.J. Cao, W.C. Wang.
The relationship between perfectionism and depressive symptoms among Chinese college students: The mediating roles of self-compassion and impostor syndrome.
Current Psychology, 42 (2023), pp. 18823-18831
[Lundh et al., 2008]
L. Lundh, F. Saboonchi, M. Wangby.
The role of personal standards in clinically significant perfectionism. A person-oriented approach to the study of patterns of perfectionism.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32 (2008), pp. 333-350
[Madigan, 2019]
D.J. Madigan.
A meta-analysis of perfectionism and academic achievement.
Educational Psychology Review, 31 (2019), pp. 967-989
[Martínez et al., 2021]
Á. M. Martínez, M.D.M. Jurado, M.C. Pérez-Fuentes, A.B.B. Martín, M.D.S Márquez, B.M.T. Martínez, M. Sisto, J.J.G. Gázquez-Linares.
Self-efficacy, positive future outlook and school burnout in Spanish adolescents.
Sustainability, 13 (2021), pp. 1-10
[Martínez-Pérez and Pérez-Martín, 2023]
J.A Martínez-Pérez, P.S. Pérez-Martín.
La Curva ROC.
Medicina en Familia, SEMERGEN, 49 (2023), pp. 1-3
[Pázozdy et al., 2023]
C. Pázozdy, J. Askew, J. Dyer, P. Gately, L. Martin, K. Mavor, G.R. Brown.
The imposter phenomenon and its relationship with self‐efficacy, perfectionism, and happiness in university students.
Current Psychology, (2023), pp. 1-10
[Postigo et al., 2021]
Á. Postigo, M. Cuesta, R. Fernández-Alonso, E. García-Cueto, J. Muñiz.
La tenacidad académica modula la evaluación del rendimiento escolar: Un análisis de transiciones latentes.
Revista de Psicodidáctica, 26 (2021), pp. 87-95
[Seo, 2008]
E.H. Seo.
Self-efficacy as a mediator in the relationship between self-oriented perfectionism and academic procrastination.
Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 36 (2008), pp. 753-764
[Seong and Chang, 2021]
H. Seong, E. Chang.
Profiles of perfectionism, achievement emotions, and academic burnout in South Korean adolescents: Testing the 2 × 2 model of Perfectionism.
Learning and Individual Differences, 90 (2021), pp. 1-12
[Serrano et al., 2022]
C. Serrano, S. Murgui, Y. Andreu.
Mejorando la predicción y comprensión del éxito académico: El rol de las facetas de personalidad y el compromiso académico.
Revista de Psicodidáctica, 27 (2022), pp. 21-28
[Smith et al., 2022]
M.M. Smith, P.L. Hewitt, S.B. Sherry, G.L. Flett, C. Ray.
Parenting behaviors and trait perfectionism: A meta-analytic test of the social expectations and social learning models.
Journal of Research in Personality, 96 (2022), pp. 1-16
[Song and Kim, 2019]
H. Song, H. Kim.
Value-based profiles of visitors to a world heritage site: The case of Suwon Hwaseong fortress (in South Korea).
Sustainability, 11 (2019), pp. 1-19
[Stornaes et al., 2019]
A.V. Stornaes, J.H. Rosenvinge, J. Sundgot-Borgen, G. Pettersen, O. Friborg.
Profiles of perfectionism among adolescents attending specialized elite - and ordinary lower secondary schools: A Norwegian cross-sectional comparative study.
Frontiers in Psychology, 10 (2019), pp. 1-12
[Vicent, Inglés, Gonzálvez, Sanmartín, Aparicio-Flores, et al., 2019]
M. Vicent, C.J. Inglés, C. Gonzálvez, R. Sanmartín, M.P. Aparicio-Flores, J.M. García-Fernández.
Perfiles de perfeccionismo y autoatribuciones causales académicas en estudiantes españoles de Educación Primaria.
Revista de Psicodidáctica, 24 (2019), pp. 103-110
[Vicent, Inglés, Gonzálvez, Sanmartín, Ortega-Sandoval, et al., 2019]
M. Vicent, C.J. Inglés, C. Gonzálvez, R. Sanmartín, V.N. Ortega-Sandoval, J.M. García-Fernández.
Testing the 2 × 2 model of perfectionism in Ecuadorian adolescent population.
Journal of Health Psychology, 25 (2019), pp. 791-797
[Vicent et al., 2017]
M. Vicent, C.J. Inglés, R. Sanmartín, C. Gonzálvez, J.M. García-Fernández.
Perfectionism and aggression: Identifying risk profiles in children.
Personality and Individual Differences, 112 (2017), pp. 106-112
[Vicent, Inglés, Sanmartín, et al., 2019]
M. Vicent, C.J. Inglés, R. Sanmartín, C. Gonzálvez, B. Delgado, J.M. García-Fernández.
Spanish validation of the Child and Adolescent Perfectionism Scale: Factorial invariance and latent means differences across sex and age.
Brain Sciences, 9 (2019), pp. 1-18
[Vicent et al., 2022]
M. Vicent, R. Sanmartín, N.I. Cargua-García, J.M. García-Fernández.
Perfectionism and emotional intelligence: A person-centered approach.
International Journal of Clinical Practice, 2022 (2022), pp. 1-9
[Vicent et al., 2021]
M. Vicent, R. Sanmartín, O. Vásconez-Rubio, J.M. García-Fernández.
Perfectionism profiles and motivation to exercise based on self-determination theory.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (2021), pp. 1-12
[Wang et al., 2023]
Y.H. Wang, Y.L. Wang, K. Misener, M. Libben.
Examining the effectiveness of cognitive bias modification for perfectionism in exploration of the mediating and moderating effects of body dissatisfaction and self-efficacy.
Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues, 42 (2023), pp. 1635-1651
[Weber and Harzer, 2022]
M. Weber, C. Harzer.
Relations between character strengths, school satisfaction, enjoyment of learning, academic self-efficacy, and school achievement: An examination of various aspects of positive schooling.
Frontiers in Psychology, 13 (2022), pp. 1-15
[Willians and Edwards, 2022]
M.N. Willians, S.R. Edwards.
Conceptual replication of Seo (2008), "Self-efficacy as a mediator in the relationship between self-oriented perfectionism and academic procrastination.
Current Issues in Personality Psychology, 10 (2022), pp. 98-111
[Wuthrich et al., 2021]
V.M. Wuthrich, J. Belcher, C. Kilby, T. Jagiello, C. Lowe.
Tracking stress, depression, and anxiety across the final year of secondary school: A longitudinal study.
Journal of School Psychology, 88 (2021), pp. 18-30
[Yang et al., 2022]
J.M. Yang, J.Y. Xu, H. Zhang.
Resiliency and academic engagement: A moderated mediation model.
Psychology in the Schools, 59 (2022), pp. 900-914
[Zeinalipour, 2022]
H. Zeinalipour.
School connectedness, academic self-efficacy, and academic performance: Mediating role of hope.
Psychological Reports, 125 (2022), pp. 2052-2068
Copyright © 2023. Universidad de País Vasco
Article options
Tools
es en pt

¿Es usted profesional sanitario apto para prescribir o dispensar medicamentos?

Are you a health professional able to prescribe or dispense drugs?

Você é um profissional de saúde habilitado a prescrever ou dispensar medicamentos