metricas
covid
Buscar en
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology
Toda la web
Inicio International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology Transdiagnostic study of dynamic brain activity and connectivity among people wi...
Información de la revista
Vol. 25. Núm. 1.
(enero - marzo 2025)
Visitas
409
Vol. 25. Núm. 1.
(enero - marzo 2025)
Acceso a texto completo
Transdiagnostic study of dynamic brain activity and connectivity among people with gambling and internet gaming disorders
Visitas
409
Hui Zhoua,1, Yuwen Heb,c,1, Lulu Liub,d, Jingwen Yinb,d,e, Anita Yingxin Xiongb,f, Ka Heng Leongb, Anise M.S. Wub,d,
Autor para correspondencia
anisewu@um.edu.mo

Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Macau, Avenida da Universidade, Taipa, Macao, China.
, Marc N. Potenzag,h
a Department of Psychology, School of Educational Sciences, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing, China
b Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Institute of Collaborative Innovation, University of Macau, Macao, China
c Bioimaging Core, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Macau, Macau, China
d Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Macau, Macao, China
e Department of Psychiatry, Affiliated Hospital of Guangdong Medical University, Zhanjiang, China
f Department of Psychology and Behavioural Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
g Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
h Connecticut Council on Problem Gambling, Wethersfield, CT, USA
Este artículo ha recibido
Información del artículo
Resumen
Texto completo
Bibliografía
Descargar PDF
Estadísticas
Figuras (6)
Mostrar másMostrar menos
Tablas (1)
Table 1. Participants’ demographics and behavioral indicators.
Tablas
Abstract

Despite both internet gaming disorder (IGD) and gambling disorder (GD) being officially recognized as medical conditions by the World Health Organization, controversies persist. A transdiagnostic study may help inform classification and intervention approaches. IGD and GD may share or have distinct neural and behavioral features. To investigate, resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and self-reported behavioral data were collected from 58 individuals with GD, 31 with IGD, and 83 healthy control (HC) participants. After controlling for demographics, both GD and IGD groups scored lower on measures of gambling-related positive play. Neural data revealed reduced brain connectivity in the right rectus/orbital frontal gyrus in GD and IGD groups compared to HC participants. IGD participants displayed increased dynamic brain activity in the left triangular inferior frontal gyrus compared with GD and HC participants. Relatively decreased modular flexibility was also observed in GD but not IGD participants, relative to HC participants. Multiclass classification results showed that the indicators of gambling-related positive play, as well as dynamic brain activity and connectivity patterns, were useful for classifying GD, IGD, and HC participants, outperforming the use of either neural signals or self-report indicators alone. The shared phenotypes of GD and IGD groups provide insight into common features of behavioral addictions, and the combination of self-report and neural measures may provide the most robust approach for classification of diagnostic groups.

Keywords:
Gambling
Video games
Addictive behaviors
Impulsive behaviors
Compulsive behaviors
Resting-state
fMRI
Texto completo
Introduction

Gambling disorder (GD) involves persistent and recurrent problematic patterns of betting that lead to clinical impairment (e.g., sleep disturbances, emotional distress, and financial and relational problems; Ciccarelli et al., 2017; Parham et al., 2012). Internet gaming disorder (IGD) is similarly characterized by persistent and recurrent engagement in video gaming leading to negative outcomes (e.g., poor sleep, emotional distress, and academic problems; Benjet et al., 2023; Bonnaire & Baptista, 2019; Wong et al., 2020). Although both GD and IGD are considered medical conditions by professional health organizations (Chinese Society of Psychiatry, 2001; World Health Organization, 2022), the latter in particular has been recently debated regarding conceptualization, diagnostic symptoms, underlying mechanism(s), and classification (Jin et al., 2019; Kuss et al., 2017; Van Rooij & Kardefelt-Winther, 2017). Research is needed to better understand its prevalence, clinical course, and neurobiological underpinnings (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2022). Common processes (e.g., dysfunctions in reward processing and inhibitory control; Antons et al., 2020) have been proposed between GD and IGD. Direct comparisons between GD and IGD provide direct insights into shared and unique elements that could help guide intervention development.

Phenotypic features constitute important elements of specific mental disorders (Courtenay et al., 2009; Cuijpers et al., 2010). Compared with healthy control (HC) participants, both individuals with GD and those with IGD have exhibited greater risk preference and higher impulsivity (Argyriou et al., 2017; Moccia et al., 2017; Schluter & Hodgins, 2021; Zhu et al., 2023). In addition, both GD and IGD have been associated with poor behavioral control (e.g., setting limits on time/money spent on playing; Costes & Bonnaire, 2022; Lavoie & Main, 2019). In GD, gambling-related positive play, as the indicator of individuals’ beliefs and behaviors referring to responsible and controlled gambling (Wood et al., 2017), has been found to be negatively correlated with GD (Tong et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2022; Zhou & Wu, 2024).

Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that people with GD and individuals with IGD exhibit shared differences from those without in brain activations during task performance and functional connectivity during resting state. For example, in studies examining dual process models using reward and control tasks, people with either GD or IGD have shown different activations from HC participants in the frontal cortex (e.g., orbital frontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex /medial frontal cortex), anterior cingulate cortex, and striatum (Crockford et al., 2005; de Ruiter et al., 2012; Ding et al., 2014; Goudriaan et al., 2010; Ko et al., 2009, 2014; Luijten et al., 2017; van Holst et al., 2012; Yao et al., 2017). Resting-state fMRI, that records spontaneous neural activity and can be used to examine functional connectivity within and between brain networks (Hohenfeld et al., 2018; Smitha et al., 2017), suggests analogous neurobiological bases of GD and IGD. For example, altered resting-state functional connectivity between regions of frontoparietal network and that of the limbic network has been observed in people with IGD (See a review; Yan et al., 2021), and functional connectivity differences in frontal areas and part of the limbic network, striatal regions, have been reported in people with versus without GD (Jung et al., 2014; Koehler et al., 2013; Tschernegg et al., 2013).

Despite similarities across GD or IGD in studies investigating each condition separately, few neuroimaging studies have directly examined GD and IGD concurrently. To the authors’ best knowledge, only one resting-state fMRI study, in which 14 participants with internet-based GD, 15 with IGD, and 15 HC participants were compared for their stationary resting-state functional connectivity in default-mode, cognitive control, and reward networks (Bae et al., 2017). Bae et al. (2017) found internet-based GD and IGD groups shared decreased functional connectivity in the default-mode network while exhibiting differences in the other two networks. Specifically, the IGD group had increased functional connectivity within the cognitive control network compared to both the GD and HC groups, and the GD group had increased functional connectivity within reward circuitry compared with both the IGD and HC groups. Further studies, especially those not limiting GD to internet-based GD, are needed as internet-based GD may not generalize to GD more broadly (Russell et al., 2022).

Bae et al. (2017)’s study applied a static analytic approach, one susceptible to loss of information contained in the temporal patterns of spontaneous signals (Hutchison et al., 2013). Large-scale dynamic neural statistics are emerging powerful methods to capture how the human brain adapts to different circumstances to conduct appropriate behaviors across time (Lurie et al., 2020). In particular, both dynamic fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (fALFF), which depicts the spontaneous brain activity (Zou et al., 2008), and dynamic degree centrality (DC), which quantifies the degree to which one brain node connects to all other nodes, are powerful dynamic neuroimaging statistics (Zuo et al., 2012) and have been used in previous studies of mental disorders (Lu et al., 2020; Sun et al., 2022).

In addition to dynamic spontaneous brain activity and connectivity, brain networks also reconfigure spontaneously to adapt to changing internal and external circumstances (Bassett et al., 2011; He et al., 2018). The multilayer network algorithm is a robust method to probe the reconfiguration of brain networks across time (Mucha et al., 2010). It helps estimate an individual's neural flexibility, which underpins cognitive flexibility (i.e., tendencies to adapt to different circumstances; Ionescu, 2012) at a behavioral level (Bassett et al., 2011; Braun et al., 2015). Previous studies have reported a positive correlation between problem-gambling severity and cognitive inflexibility (Leppink et al., 2016), which was also found in patients with either GD (Álvarez-Moya et al., 2009; Perandrés-Gómez et al., 2021) or IGD (Kim et al., 2017). These results suggest potential altered neural flexibility in people with GD and IGD.

Although there have been efforts using machine learning to explore the use of behavioral and neural markers to statistically classify GD (Genauck et al., 2020, 2021) or IGD (Stavropoulos et al., 2023; Wang et al., 2022), no study to our knowledge has used both phenotypes simultaneously to distinguish GD and IGD from HC groups. The combination of behavioral and neural data, which can provide a more comprehensive picture of distinct and reliable phenotypes of disorders, has been adopted in machine learning to diagnose medical conditions (e.g., Alzheimer's disease; Mirzaei & Adeli, 2022). Thus, a multiclass classification algorithm could be adopted to probe if the neural dynamic properties and controlled behavioral indicators could facilitate the successful classifying of GD and IGD. The findings could potentially be useful in assisting disease classification and clinical diagnosis and to advance interventions.

In this study, we adopted a transdiagnostic approach (Fusar‐Poli et al., 2019), intending to identify similarities across IGD and GD and differences across the disorders. Gambling-related positive play, as one of the potential behavioral phenotypes referring to responsible and controlled gambling, was examined among GD and IGD groups. Dynamic statistics were used to probe potential alternations in neural dynamics in GD and IGD using resting-state fMRI data to identify shared and specific dysfunctional brain circuits. Advanced analytic approaches probed neural flexibility in individuals with GD and IGD using a multilayer network algorithm. The multiclass classification algorithm was used to explore whether these neural and/or behavioral indicators could be helpful for GD and IGD classification.

MethodsParticipants and procedures

All participants were Chinese adults aged 18 years or above and were recruited via posts on social media, posters and personal referral in the corresponding author's university and local community centers. If individuals were interested in study participation and provided their contact information either in an online form or verbally via personal phone calls or messages, they were scheduled by the trained research assistant for intake involving a clinical interview followed by a self-reported questionnaire survey and an MRI scan. All participants were interviewed by a psychiatrist before MRI scanning. The diagnostic inclusion criteria were based on the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-5;APA, 2013). The exclusion criteria for participants included any self-reported physical disease, psychosis due to neurologic or medical conditions, especially schizophrenia spectrum and other psychotic disorders, manic episodes, bipolar disorder, depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, and substance use disorders, as well as the history of using dopaminergic medications associated with GD (e.g., levodopa, pramipexole, ropinirole and aripiprazole) and family psychosis. According to the psychiatrist's diagnostic assessment of participants’ past-year gambling/gaming experience, 58 participants were diagnosed with GD, 31 with IGD, and 83 with neither (i.e., HC participants), after removing six participants from further analyses due to incomplete data.

Before clinical interviews, all participants provided written consent. All participants completed a questionnaire with items regarding their demographics (age, sex) and responsible and controlled play levels, which were assessed by the Chinese version (Tong et al., 2020) of the Positive Play Scale (Wood et al., 2017). The scale consists of a 7-item beliefs subscale (e.g., “I should be aware of how much money I spend when I gamble”) and a 7-item behaviors subscale (e.g., “I was honest with my family and/or friends about the amount of time I spent on gambling”). Participants answered on a 5-point Likert scale (from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). The higher total score in both beliefs and behaviors scales represented a higher level of corresponding responsible and controlled gambling.

The study was reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Committee at the corresponding author's university (reference number: BSERE20-APP014-ICI-01).

MRI acquisition

MRI scans were obtained by a 3T Siemens Magnetom Prisma scanner at the University of Macau. The MRI scanning parameters were the same as in a previous study (Zhou et al., 2023). The parameters for the structural scan were FOV = 256 mm; slice number = 176; TR = 2300 ms; TE = 2.26 ms; slice thickness = 1 mm. A resting-state fMRI scan for each participant lasted 10 min and consisted of 600 vol. Participants were asked to fixate on a white cross with a black background and not to focus on any thoughts. The scanning parameters for resting-state fMRI were FoV = 192 mm; slice number = 65; slice thickness = 2 mm; TR = 1000 ms; TE = 30 ms; phase encoding direction = AP; flip angle = 60.

MRI data preprocessing

A MATLAB-based MRI data processing package DPABI (Yan et al., 2016), the preprocessing procedures of which are sourced from statistical parametric mapping (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/), was used to conduct the preprocessing procedures of fMRI data. To avoid the instability of the MRI scanner, the first 10 vol were removed from the original 600 vol. Realignment was performed to address head motion. Several confounding effects were removed for further analyses, including constant, linear, and quadratic trends (Power et al., 2012), 24 head motion parameters (Friston et al., 1996), white matter and cerebral fluid measures, and regressors relating to head motion. Further procedures included normalizing the functional space into a standard MNI space and smoothing the data with a 4 mm Gaussian kernel. In addition, a bandpass filter (0.01 ∼0.1) was adopted to remove noise for the calculation of DC and functional connectivity between pairs of regions of interest (ROIs).

Temporal variation of spontaneous brain activity

The fALFF summarizes the relative power magnitude of a designated frequency range (0.01–0.1 Hz) in spontaneous brain activity (Zou et al., 2008). The full scanning volumes were divided into sliding windows of 30 time points each, with 1 sliding step as recommended as the default parameters by a previous study (Yan et al., 2017), resulting in 561 windows in total for each participant. The fALFF was calculated for each voxel for each participant in each time window. The standard deviation of fALFF across time was calculated to depict the temporal variation of spontaneous brain activity of each voxel.

Temporal variations in brain connectivity

The DC is an index of the overall degree of connecting strength of one brain node to all other brain nodes (Zuo et al., 2012). The full scanning volumes were divided into sliding windows of 50 time points each, with 5 sliding steps. While Hutchison et al.’s review (2013) has identified window length of 30s–60 s bringing the most robust results, they have also highlighted, a too long window length must be avoided to capture the dynamic fluctuations of brain connectivity across time despite the fact that a longer window length can make the connectivity results more stable. Consistent with previous research (Sun et al., 2022; Yang et al., 2022), 50 TRs were therefore, adopted as the window length. In addition, although both 5 TRs and 1 TR were the common sliding step in previous studies (Demirtaş et al., 2016; Espinoza et al., 2019; Pedersen et al., 2018), a longer sliding step (i.e., 5 TRs) was adopted to avoid overloading computational resources for the calculation of dynamic DC, resulting in 109 windows in total for each participant. DC was calculated for each voxel for each participant in each time window. The standard deviation of DC across time was calculated to depict the temporal variation of DC of each voxel.

Further functional analysis would take the clusters showing between-group effects as seeds to calculate the whole-brain-wise functional connectivity in a sliding window of 50 time points, with five sliding steps. The standard deviation of functional connectivity in the resulting 109 time windows was then calculated to index the temporal variance of functional connectivity for each voxel.

Between-group statistical analyses for behavioral measurements

One-way ANOVA was performed to examine the overall between-group effects of gambling-related positive play beliefs and behaviors in GD, IGD, and HC groups. In addition, the t-test was used to examine the differences in these measurements between each pair of groups. To avoid misinterpretation, linear covariate effects of age and sex were removed from the original scores of gambling-related positive play beliefs and behaviors to obtain normalized scores. The normalized scores of positive play beliefs and positive play behaviors were fed to repeat the abovementioned statistical analyses.

Anor: normalized score; Aorig: original score; V: vector of age, sex, or all-ones vector.

Between-group statistical analyses for voxel-wise dynamic statistics

One-way ANOVA was performed for both temporal variations of fALFF and DC among three diagnostic groups, controlling for covariates of age and sex. The significant results should survive family-wise error correction (FWE) for multiple comparisons (uncorrected p < 0.001, cluster-level FWE q < 0.05). In addition, an ROI analysis was performed to examine differences between each pair of groups in the brain regions showing whole-brain-wise between-group effects.

After controlling the covariate effects of age and sex, t-test was used to examine the differential effects of dynamic functional connectivity between each pair of groups, such as between the GD and HC groups. The same FWE approach was used to correct for multiple comparisons. Further ROI analysis was performed to understand the differential effects between each pair of groups in each brain region showing between-group effects in the whole-brain-wise exploratory analysis.

In addition, correlation analyses were performed to examine whether the dynamic activity or connectivity would be correlated with positive play scores across all groups. Further linear regression analyses were performed to examine whether the group would moderate the associations between such neural and behavioral features.

Beh: positive play beliefs or positive play behaviors; Vol: neural signals (dynamic fALFF or dynamic DC); GroupID: GD, IGD, and HC were assigned as different categorical values.

Multilayer network

A multilayer network algorithm can be used to capture the flexible modular organization of brain regions across time (Bassett et al., 2011). As shown in the following formula, the spatial resolution parameter γ controls the average number of brain regions to form a module (network), while the temporal resolution parameter ω controls how similar the organization is among different time windows. As suggested by previous studies, both γ and ω were set as 1 in the cost function (Bassett et al., 2011, 2013; Finc et al., 2020). Of note, both positive and negative connections were allowed to be incorporated to the connections with different directions. Only sequential connections between nearby windows (layers) were allowed in the cost function.

Denotes: γ regulates the spatial resolution, while ω is a temporal regulator controlling the between-layers consistency. Kis+(Kis) indicates the node degree of positive (negative) connections of node i in layer s. m+ (m) is the sum degree of layer s. δ is a delta function, and the δ function equals 1 when gis and gjs are assigned to the same module. μ is the total degree of all layers (time windows).

For the clustering of brain regions, the full length of the images of each participant was divided into sliding time windows of 50 time points in length with 1 sliding step, consistent with previous studies (Favaretto et al., 2022; Tang et al., 2022; Yang et al., 2022), which resulted in 541 time windows for each participant. The brain regions depicted in the Brainnetome atlas (Fan et al., 2016) were used to extract the average time series for each ROI in each time window, followed by the calculation of the Pearson correlation between each pair of brain regions in each time window. Then a 246 * 246 * 541 matrix for each participant was fed to make an estimate of the modular organization across time windows for each participant.

Modular flexibility

Modular flexibility was calculated as the number of times a brain region changed its module belonging in successive windows, divided by the total number of time windows (Bassett et al., 2011). To better summarize the flexibility results at a network level, the average flexibility of 8 brain networks (i.e., visual network, somatomotor network, dorsal attention network, ventral attention network, limbic network, frontoparietal network, and default mode network plus a subcortical network; Thomas Yeo et al., 2011) was calculated for between-group comparisons. Of note, the correspondence between a brain region and Yeo's brain network can be retrieved from the webpage of Brainntome atlas (https://atlas.brainnetome.org/download.html). Then the between-group effects were examined for each network's flexibility with a two-sample t-test after controlling for age and sex. Multiple comparisons were corrected by false discovery rate (FDR).

Multiclass classification

The scores of beliefs and behaviors of positive play, dynamic fALFF and DC in the brain regions showing group differences, and modular flexibility were used as phenotypes to distinguish among GD, IGD, and HC participants. A naïve Bayesian classifier was used to train the data, followed by a 5-fold cross-validation process to estimate prediction accuracy. The classification process was conducted by an open-sourced MATLAB package (Treder, 2020).

ResultsGroup comparisons for the demographic and behavioral data

There were significant differences in both age (p < 0.001) and sex (p = 0.01) among groups (Table 1). The average age of GD participants was 37.67 years, IGD participants was 24.48 years, and HC participants was 27.31 years. While GD and HC groups were male-predominant, the IGD group was more closely balanced. The average score of beliefs of positive play was 22.93 in GD, 29.26 in IGD, and 28.57 in HC participants. Regarding behaviors of positive play, the average score was 19.43 in GD, 28.22 in IGD, and 28.77 in HC participants. The ANOVA analyses revealed significant between-group effects regardless of covariate effects of age and sex were controlled (positive play beliefs: F = 8.60, p < 0.001; positive play behaviors: F = 20.73, p < 0.001) or not (positive play beliefs: F = 35.48, p < 0.001; positive play behaviors: F = 60.86, p < 0.001). Specifically, original scores of these beliefs/behaviors in GD group were significantly lower than those in both IGD and HC groups (t > 6.75, p < 0.001). However, there were significant negative correlations between age and positive play beliefs (r = −0.43, p < 0.001) and behaviors (r = −0.37, p < 0.001), and males had lower levels of positive play beliefs (Mmale = 25.70, Mfemale = 28.00, t = −2.50, p = 0.01) and behaviors (Mmale = 23.36, Mfemale = 28.47, t = −4.33, p < 0.001) than females. Thus, as shown in Fig. 1, after controlling covariate effects of age and sex, both GD and IGD groups displayed significantly lower scores of both positive beliefs/behaviors than those in the HC group (t > 3, p < 0.001).

Table 1.

Participants’ demographics and behavioral indicators.

  GD  IGD  HC  p2/F) 
Age (mean ± SD)  37.67±11.99  24.48±4.95  27.31±9.68  < 0.001(25.24) 
Sex (M/F)  49/9  17/14  58/25  0.01(9.22) 
Onset of GD or IGD  23.29 ± 8.12  11.80 ± 10.56  –  – 
Duration of GD or IGD  14.72 ± 5.20  12.16 ± 6.64  –  – 
Positive play beliefs  22.93 ± 3.65  29.26 ± 4.10  28.57 ± 4.25  < 0.001(35.48) 
Positive play behaviors  19.43 ± 4.92  28.22 ± 5.01  28.77 ± 4.51  < 0.001(60.86) 

Note: GD = Gambling disorder; IGD = Internet gaming disorder; HC = Healthy control.

Fig. 1.

The normalized results of self-reported variables.

Note: GD = Gambling disorder; IGD = Internet gaming disorder; HC = Healthy control. ⁎⁎p < 0.01, ⁎⁎⁎p < 0.001.

(0.22MB).
Temporal variations in spontaneous brain activity and connectivity

The whole-brain analysis revealed between-group effects of dynamic fALFF in the left triangular inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Fig. 2A, uncorrected p < 0.001, cluster-level q < 0.05). Further ROI analysis indicated that the variation of dynamic fALFF in IGD participants was higher than those in GD and HC participants (Fig. 2C, p < 0.01). Meanwhile, whole-brain analysis revealed between-group effects of dynamic DC in the right rectus/orbital frontal gyrus (Fig. 2B, uncorrected p < 0.001, cluster-level q < 0.05). Further ROI analysis indicated the DC of both GD and IGD participants was significantly lower than that of HC participants (Fig. 2D, p < 0.01).

Fig. 2.

Temporal variations in brain activation and connectivity among GD, IGD, and HC participants. A: The left IFG showed significant group effects of dynamic fALFF; B: A bar graph displaying the results of post-hoc ROI analysis of dynamic fALFF; C: The right rectus/orbital frontal gyrus showed significant group effects of dynamic DC; D: A bar graph displaying the results of post-hoc ROI analysis of dynamic DC.

Note: GD = Gambling disorder; IGD = Internet gaming disorder; HC = Healthy control; IFG = Inferior frontal gyrus; ROI = Regions of interest; fALFF = Fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation; DC = Degree centrality. ⁎⁎p < 0.01. The whole-brain-wise significant threshold is FWE cluster-level q < 0.05, uncorrected p < 0.001.

(0.41MB).

Correlation analyses revealed that dynamic fALFF had a significant correlation with positive play beliefs (Fig. 3A, r = 0.19, p = 0.03) and a positive, although statistically non-significant, correlation with positive play behaviors (r = 0.15, p = 0.09) across all groups. In contrast, dynamic DC did not have any significant correlations with positive play scores across all groups (positive play beliefs: r = −0.05, p = 0.56; positive play behaviors: r = 0.01, p = 0.89). Further linear regression analyses demonstrated that the group (i.e., GD, IGD, and HC) significantly moderated the associations between dynamic activity and positive play scores (Fig. 3B and C) as well as dynamic connectivity and positive play scores (Fig. 3D and E).

Fig. 3.

Associations between dynamic brain features and self-reported behavioral data. A: Correlations between dynamic brain activity and positive play beliefs across all groups; B/C: The moderated effect of the group in the association between dynamic brain activity and positive play beliefs/behaviors; D/E: The moderated effect of the group in the association between dynamic brain connectivity and positive play beliefs/behaviors.

Note: *p < 0.05, ⁎⁎⁎p < 0.001.

(0.29MB).
Temporal variations in functional connectivity

As shown in Fig. 4A and B, when compared to HC, people with GD had higher temporal variability of functional connectivity between the right orbital frontal gyrus and target regions including bilateral IFG, bilateral middle frontal gyrus, right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, right inferior temporal gyrus and superior temporal pole, right inferior parietal lobule, bilateral supramarginal gyrus, and right precuneus, while people with IGD had higher temporal variability of functional connectivity between the right orbital frontal gyrus and the right lingual gyrus. Further ROI analysis illustrated a more detailed comparison between each pair of groups, indicating significant differences between GD and HC groups, narrow differences between IGD and HC groups, and barely any differences between GD and IGD groups (Fig. 4C).

Fig. 4.

The between-group effects of temporal variability of functional connectivity of the right OFG. A: The brain regions showing higher temporal variability of functional connectivity with the right OFG in the GD or IGD groups when compared to the HC group; B: The connections with the right OFG showing higher temporal variability of functional connectivity with the right OFG in the GD (blue) or IGD (green) group when compared to the HC group; C: A bar graph for comparisons of temporal variation of functional connectivity with the right OFG in 12 ROIs among three groups.

Note: GD = Gambling disorder; IGD = Internet gaming disorder; HC = Healthy control. l = Left; r = Right; OFG = Orbital frontal gyrus; IFG = Inferior frontal gyrus; MFG = Middle frontal gyrus; DLPFC = Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; ITG = Inferior temporal gyrus; STP = Superior temporal pole; IPL = Inferior parietal lobule; SupraM = Supramarginal gyrus; Precu = Precuneus; Ling = Lingual gyrus; ROI= region of interest.

(0.39MB).
Modular flexibility

The average modular flexibility across the brain for each group is shown in Fig. 5. Controlling for age and sex, the GD group had significantly lower flexibility in all 8 brain networks when compared to HC participants (FDR q < 0.05), while the IGD group did not reveal any significant differences in modular flexibility compared to HC participants. The IGD group showed higher network flexibility in subcortical, somatomotor, dorsal and ventral attention, and frontoparietal networks when compared to the GD group (p < 0.05, FDR q > 0.05).

Fig. 5.

Modular flexibility among GD, IGD, and HC groups. A: Average raw modular flexibility in the GD group; B: Average raw modular flexibility in the IGD group; C: Average raw modular flexibility in the HC group; D: The normalized modular flexibility after controlling for age and sex effects in the GD, IGD, and HC groups, as well as the significant levels of between-group comparisons.

Note: GD = Gambling disorder; IGD = Internet gaming disorder; HC = Healthy control. Sub = Subcortical network; Vis = Visual network; SMN = Somatomotor network; DAN = Dorsal attention network; VAN = Ventral attention network, Lim = Limbic network; FPN = Frontoparietal network; DMN = Default-mode network. *p < 0.05, ⁎⁎p < 0.01, ⁎⁎⁎p < 0.001. All p < 0.01 survive false discovery rate correction q < 0.05.

(0.5MB).
Multiclass classification

Positive play scores and dynamic fALFF and DC measures could distinguish among GD, IGD, and HC groups (Fig. 6A, 5B). Combining measures of the beliefs and behaviors of positive play, dynamic fALFF, and dynamic DC made the best diagnostic classification of GD and IGD, with an accuracy of 72.30 %, while using behavioral or neural data alone reached lower accuracies (see Fig. 6C). Modular flexibility had little power in predicting group assignments (accuracy: 37.93, Fig. 6C). Combining phenotypes of modular flexibility with phenotypes of positive play and dynamic fALFF, dynamic DC decreased the predictive power to 58.67 % accuracy.

Fig. 6.

Multiclass classification for the GD, IGD, and HC participants. A: A 3-D scatter plot of the data points of each individual in the dimensions of dynamic fALFF, dynamic DC, and positive play beliefs; B: A 3-D scatter plot of the data points of each individual in the dimensions of dynamic fALFF, dynamic DC, and positive play behaviors; C: The predicting accuracy of diagnosis of GD, IGD, or HC with different sets of phenotypes including phenotypes of only behaviors (Only-beh), of dynamic fALFF and DC (DC-fALFF), of behaviors, dynamic fALFF, and dynamic DC (Beh-DC-fALFF), of modular flexibility (Flexibility), or all mentioned above (all).

Note: GD = Gambling disorder; IGD = Internet gaming disorder; HC = Healthy control; fALFF = Fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation; DC = Degree centrality.

(0.17MB).
Discussion

The present transdiagnostic study is the first to compare dynamic brain activity among GD, IGD, and HC groups. Additionally, self-reported measures of gambling-related positive play were compared for the first time among these three groups. To summarize, (a) compared with HC participants, those with GD or IGD showed not only lower levels of beliefs and behaviors of gambling-related positive play after controlling for covariate effects of age and sex, but also relatively decreased dynamic brain connectivity in the right rectus/orbital frontal gyrus; (b) the dynamic brain activity in the left triangular IFG was increased in IGD but not GD participants, while the modular flexibility was decreased in GD but not IGD participants; (c) the classification of GD, IGD, and HC groups reached high accuracy with making use of self-reported (i.e., beliefs and behaviors of positive play) and neural (i.e., dynamic fALFF and DC) data.

Shared characteristics of GD and IGD groups

Compared with HC, both GD and IGD groups reported lower levels of beliefs and behaviors regarding the regulation of behaviors after controlling for the effects of age and sex. Consistent with previous studies showing the negative correlation between GD and gambling-related positive play in community-dwelling individuals who gamble (Tong et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2022; Zhou & Wu, 2024), this study provided empirical evidence regarding low levels of self-reported responsible and controlled gambling among individuals with GD. At the same time, the study also found that people with IGD had lower levels of self-reported responsible and controlled gambling when controlling for effects of age and sex, even though the unadjusted scores were more similar to the HC group. These findings raise the possibility that responsible and low-risk gambling may show a complex relationship with IGD. However, future studies are warranted to recruit demographically matched participants to investigate further differences in gambling-related positive play between IGD and HC groups, as well as to determine the extent to which individuals with IGD may demonstrate gaming-related decrements in positive play.

The present study found GD and IGD participants shared decreased dynamic brain connectivity in the right rectus/orbital frontal gyrus. The orbital frontal gyrus, including the rectus gyrus, has been implicated in value representation, reward processing, and decision-making (Rolls, 2004), and differences in the reward and value systems (e.g., hypersensitivity to addiction-related rewards, blunted responses to monetary reward anticipation and more risky choices) in GD and IGD have been reported (Hewig et al., 2010; Luijten et al., 2017; Raiha et al., 2020; Wiehler & Peters, 2015). Consistent with several studies revealing dysfunction in the orbital frontal gyrus during decision-making in GD (Brand et al., 2005; Freinhofer et al., 2020), the altered dynamic brain connections in the right orbital frontal gyrus might be one of salient neural factors contributing to disturbed reward and value processes in GD and IGD. Among the limited studies examining brain dynamic connectivity in GD or IGD, one study reported the altered dynamic interactions within frontal-striatal circuits (e.g., inhibitory effective connectivity from the orbital frontal gyrus to the right caudate) in IGD, supporting abnormal reward processing among individuals with behavioral addictions (Zeng et al., 2022). Thus, our results echo the existing literature on involvement of the orbital frontal gyrus in GD and IGD and provide additional knowledge relating to altered dynamic connectivity between the orbital frontal gyrus and other brain regions. Further dynamic functional connectivity analysis revealed that many distributed brain regions reported by a meta-analysis showing altered grey matter in addictive disorders (Zhang et al., 2021) had higher temporal variability of functional connectivity with the right orbital frontal gyrus, further implicating the right orbital frontal gyrus in both GD and IGD groups.

Decreased levels of beliefs and behaviors of responsible and controlled play in GD and IGD groups after controlling for covariate effects of age and sex suggest possible deficits in awareness and ability to regulate and control their own gambling, with impaired impulse control considered as a core feature of addictions (Weinberg, 2013). The GD and IGD groups shared altered dynamic brain connectivity in the right rectus/orbital frontal gyrus, of which these two groups also had higher temporal variability of functional connectivity with other brain regions compared to HC group. Such findings resonate with previous studies that have found that stimulating brain regions with strong connections with the orbital frontal gyrus may disrupt outcome-guided behaviors (Howard et al., 2020). As such, the orbital frontal gyrus may be a key brain region in reward-based decision-making processes in behavioral addiction (Yalcinbas et al., 2021). Thus, the right rectus/orbital frontal gyrus is recommended as one of the core brain regions in future studies for evaluating the effectiveness of neuro-stimulation therapies (e.g., transcranial magnetic stimulation) in behavioral addiction interventions, either directly or through connected brain regions.

Differences between GD and IGD groups

The dynamic brain activity in the left triangular IFG was higher among IGD than GD and HC participants. This finding was in line with the results of a meta-analysis that showed activity differences in the left IFG among people with IGD, possibly relating to poor executive functioning or impulse control among individuals with IGD (Zheng et al., 2019). Despite some studies having reported the involvement of IFG in GD (Miedl et al., 2015), there has been arguably less consistent evidence concerning IFG functional differences in individuals with GD during executive or general performance (Quaglieri et al., 2020; Raimo et al., 2021). Therefore, the altered dynamic brain activity in IFG may be more consistently linked to difficulties in executive functioning or impulse control in individuals with IGD. In addition, the group moderated the associations between dynamic brain activity as well as connectivity and positive play scores, suggesting that the brain-behavior association is not simply a linear relationship across the disorders. These patterns of brain-behavior associations might differ in individuals with GD or IGD, providing a potential way to distinguish GD and IGD based on such brain-behavior association patterns.

The dynamic functional connectivity analysis revealed more brain regions showing a higher temporal variation of functional connectivity with the right orbital frontal gyrus in the GD group when compared to HC participants, whereas only a narrower portion of brain regions showed a higher temporal variation of functional connectivity in the IGD group compared to the HC group. Higher temporal variation of functional connectivity between the right orbital frontal gyrus and these brain regions might suggest that there are more fluctuating information exchanges affecting value encoding as the orbital frontal gyrus is an important brain region for reward processing and decision-making (Rolls, 2004). The more pervasively higher temporal variation of functional connectivity with the right orbital frontal gyrus in the GD group might suggest people with GD have more pervasive deficits in the reward processes or decision-making, while the people with IGD might have a more tempered deficit in reward processing or decision-making networks, although these possibilities are currently speculative and warrant direct examination.

Previous studies, including a meta-analysis, have found cognitive inflexibility among people with GD (Grant & Chamberlain, 2023; Leppink et al., 2016; Perandrés-Gómez et al., 2021; Van Timmeren et al., 2018). Considering that brain network flexibility may function as the neural underpinning of one's cognitive flexibility at a behavioral level, with the brain network flexibility positively correlating with cognitive flexibility (Bassett et al., 2011; Braun et al., 2015), the current study suggesting neural inflexibility in GD may relate to cognitive inflexibility, and this possibility warrants direct examination. Furthermore, as no altered network flexibility was observed among the IGD versus HC participants, it suggests that alternate mechanisms may be operating in IGD versus GD. Despite cognitive inflexibility (i.e., reflected by the Stroop interference effect) having been reported previously in individuals with IGD (Dong et al., 2015; Kim et al., 2017), the results suggest that some aspects of cognitive flexibility at a neural level may be preserved in IGD. Other studies have noted the enhancement of multitasking and task-switching abilities through online/video games (Anguera et al., 2013; Choi et al., 2021; Ryu et al., 2021), which speculatively may relate to preserved network flexibility in IGD. These results taken together suggest that individuals with IGD may experience deficits in some types of cognitive flexibility (e.g., space rotation flexibility; Podlogar & Podlesek, 2022) but not others. In the context of previous results, our findings also suggest that individuals with GD might show more pervasive deficits of cognitive flexibility than those with IGD, although this speculative possibility warrants future direct examination. For example, further studies are warranted to examine whether individuals with GD exhibit more network inflexibility than individuals with IGD during task performance.

Benefits of combining neural and self-report indicators for GD and IGD classification

To the best of our knowledge, the present study is the first to adopt multiclass classification, based on both neural and self-reported data, to explore the accuracy of simultaneously classifying GD, IGD, and HC participants. Several prior studies have used brain or non-neuroimaging data to perform classification analysis to distinguish either GD and HC (Genauck et al., 2020, 2021) or IGD and HC (Stavropoulos et al., 2023; Wang et al., 2022) groups. However, their predicting power stems from the differences between people with addictive disorders and their healthy counterparts, and do not consider alterations across different kinds of behavioral addictions (i.e., GD and IGD).

Our results showed that the combination of self-reported data (i.e., gambling-related beliefs and behaviors regarding responsible and controlled play) and dynamic fALFF and DC could reach 72.30 % accuracy in distinguishing GD, IGD, and HC groups, while incorporating the phenotypes of modular flexibility for the multiclass diagnoses decreased the accuracy to 58.67 %, possibly due to similar levels of network flexibility between IGD and HC participants. Our findings suggest that these self-report and neural indicators may constitute relevant distinguishing characteristics in GD and IGD. Thus, the combination of both self-report and neural phenotypes may provide a foundation for developing and facilitating approaches for accurate and reliable GD and IGD classification in the future.

Study limitations exist. First, a relatively small number of participants, especially female participants, were recruited, thus limiting the generalization of our results. Although the sex and age distribution indicated the current sample was a relatively representative one for GD and IGD (e.g., both the GD and IGD groups contained more men than women; Marraudino et al., 2022; Subramaniam et al., 2015), future studies are suggested to recruit larger samples with similar male-to-female ratios and wider ranges of age and ethnicity to investigate the reproducibility and generalizability of these results. Second, some potential confounders were not collected (e.g., emotional distress and the frequency of drinking/smoking), and these may impact dynamic brain activity and connectivity. Despite the exclusion of individuals with other psychiatric conditions (e.g., depressive, anxiety and substance use disorders), future studies may consider these factors and evaluate or control for their potential effects. Third, the present study did not incorporate gaming-specific positive play, with a scale having been developed and published after data collection in the present project (He et al., 2024a, 2024b). The use of this and other scales (e.g., those evaluating general impulse control or reward and punishment sensitivities) are recommended in future studies to examine further how best to use brain and behavioral data to distinguish GD, IGD, and HC participants. Fourth, our classification procedures might pose a data leakage problem (Kapoor & Narayanan, 2023), which might exaggerate the prediction performance of our study. Considering the sample sizes across groups, such procedures were adopted to provide preliminary evidence for supporting that the combined use of both neural and behavioral features may assist in the diagnosis of GD and IGD. However, future studies may take more comprehensive procedures by selecting features in a separate training set.

In conclusion, this transdiagnostic study is the first to compare the dynamic neural activity and self-reported gambling-related positive play measures among GD, IGD, and HC participants. Results revealed that GD and IGD groups shared altered dynamic brain connectivity and, after controlling for sex and age, had reduced levels of gambling-related responsible and controlled play, suggesting possible transdiagnostic features across different types of addictive disorders. Also, these two groups had disorder-specific patterns of differences in dynamic brain activity and network flexibility. In addition, using neural and self-reported measures achieved high accuracy in distinguishing GD, IGD, and HC participants, which may offer a potentially useful approach to assist in classifying GD and IGD.

Ethics approval

The procedures of this study were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The ethics approval was obtained from the affiliated university of the corresponding author.

Consent to participate

Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analysed in the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Hui Zhou: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Yuwen He: Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Lulu Liu: Writing – review & editing. Jingwen Yin: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. Anita Yingxin Xiong: Investigation, Project administration. Ka Heng Leong: Investigation, Project administration. Anise M.S. Wu: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition. Marc N. Potenza: Writing – review & editing.

Acknowledgments

The project was supported by the research grants of the University of Macau [grant numbers MYRG2022-00130-FSS, MYRG-CRG2022-00003-FSS-ICI, CRG2020-00001-ICI, MYRG-GRG2023-00074-FSS]. The funding source had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication. This work was performed in part at SICC, which is supported by SKL-IOTSC, University of Macau.

References
[Álvarez-Moya et al., 2009]
E.M. Álvarez-Moya, S. Jiménez-Murcia, L. Moragas, M. Gómez-Peña, M.N. Aymamí, C. Ochoa, et al.
Executive functioning among female pathological gambling and bulimia nervosa patients: Preliminary findings.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15 (2009), pp. 302-306
[American Psychiatric Association 2013]
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC.
[American Psychiatric Association 2022]
American Psychiatric Association. (2022). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders ((Text Revision, 5th ed.). Washington, DC. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425787.
[Anguera et al., 2013]
J.A. Anguera, J. Boccanfuso, J.L. Rintoul, O. Al-Hashimi, F. Faraji, J. Janowich, et al.
Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults.
Nature, 501 (2013), pp. 97-101
[Antons et al., 2020]
S. Antons, M. Brand, M.N. Potenza.
Neurobiology of cue-reactivity, craving, and inhibitory control in non-substance addictive behaviors.
Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 415 (2020),
[Argyriou et al., 2017]
E. Argyriou, C.B. Davison, T.T.C. Lee.
Response inhibition and internet gaming disorder: A meta-analysis.
Addictive Behaviors, 71 (2017), pp. 54-60
[Bae et al., 2017]
S. Bae, D.H. Han, J. Jung, K.C. Nam, P.F. Renshaw.
Comparison of brain connectivity between internet gambling disorder and internet gaming disorder: A preliminary study.
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6 (2017), pp. 505-515
[Bassett et al., 2013]
D.S. Bassett, M.A. Porter, N.F. Wymbs, S.T. Grafton, J.M. Carlson, P.J. Mucha.
Robust detection of dynamic community structure in networks.
Chaos: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Nonlinear Science, 23 (2013),
[Bassett et al., 2011]
D.S. Bassett, N.F. Wymbs, M.A. Porter, P.J. Mucha, J.M. Carlson, S.T. Grafton.
Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain networks during learning.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108 (2011), pp. 7641-7646
[Benjet et al., 2023]
C. Benjet, R. Orozco, Y.C. Albor, E.V. Contreras, I.R. Monroy-Velasco, P.C. Hernández Uribe, et al.
A longitudinal study on the impact of Internet gaming disorder on self-perceived health, academic performance, and social life of first-year college students.
The American Journal on Addictions, 32 (2023), pp. 343-351
[Bonnaire and Baptista, 2019]
C. Bonnaire, D. Baptista.
Internet gaming disorder in male and female young adults: The role of alexithymia, depression, anxiety and gaming type.
Psychiatry Research, 272 (2019), pp. 521-530
[Brand et al., 2005]
M. Brand, E. Kalbe, K. Labudda, E. Fujiwara, J. Kessler, H.J. Markowitsch.
Decision-making impairments in patients with pathological gambling.
Psychiatry Research, 133 (2005), pp. 91-99
[Braun et al., 2015]
U. Braun, A. Schäfer, H. Walter, S. Erk, N. Romanczuk-Seiferth, L. Haddad, et al.
Dynamic reconfiguration of frontal brain networks during executive cognition in humans.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112 (2015), pp. 11678-11683
[Chinese Society of Psychiatry 2001]
Chinese Society of Psychiatry.
CCMD-3 chinese classification of mental disorders.
3rd ed., Shandong Science and Technology Press, (2001),
[Choi et al., 2021]
E. Choi, S.-H. Shin, J.-K. Ryu, K.-I. Jung, Y. Hyun, J. Kim, et al.
Association of extensive video gaming and cognitive function changes in brain-imaging studies of pro gamers and individuals with gaming disorder: Systematic literature review.
JMIR Serious Games, 9 (2021), pp. e25793
[Ciccarelli et al., 2017]
M. Ciccarelli, M.D. Griffiths, G. Nigro, M. Cosenza.
Decision making, cognitive distortions and emotional distress: A comparison between pathological gamblers and healthy controls.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 54 (2017), pp. 204-210
[Costes and Bonnaire, 2022]
J.-M. Costes, C. Bonnaire.
Spending money in free-to-play games: Sociodemographic characteristics, motives, impulsivity and internet gaming disorder specificities.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19 (2022), pp. 15709
[Courtenay et al., 2009]
K. Courtenay, S. Soni, A. Strydom, J. Turk.
Behavioural phenotypes and mental disorders.
Psychiatry, 8 (2009), pp. 391-397
[Crockford et al., 2005]
D.N. Crockford, B. Goodyear, J. Edwards, J. Quickfall, N. el-Guebaly.
Cue-induced brain activity in pathological gamblers.
Biological Psychiatry, 58 (2005), pp. 787-795
[Cuijpers et al., 2010]
P. Cuijpers, J. Li, S.G. Hofmann, G. Andersson.
Self-reported versus clinician-rated symptoms of depression as outcome measures in psychotherapy research on depression: A meta-analysis.
Clinical Psychology Review, 30 (2010), pp. 768-778
[de Ruiter et al., 2012]
M.B. de Ruiter, J. Oosterlaan, D.J. Veltman, W. van den Brink, A.E. Goudriaan.
Similar hyporesponsiveness of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in problem gamblers and heavy smokers during an inhibitory control task.
Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 121 (2012), pp. 81-89
[Demirtaş et al., 2016]
M. Demirtaş, C. Tornador, C. Falcón, M. López-Solà, R. Hernández-Ribas, J. Pujol, et al.
Dynamic functional connectivity reveals altered variability in functional connectivity among patients with major depressive disorder.
Human Brain Mapping, 37 (2016), pp. 2918-2930
[Ding et al., 2014]
W. Ding, J. Sun, Y. Sun, X. Chen, Y. Zhou, Z. Zhuang, et al.
Trait impulsivity and impaired prefrontal impulse inhibition function in adolescents with internet gaming addiction revealed by a Go/No-Go fMRI study.
Behavioral and Brain Functions, 10 (2014), pp. 20
[Dong et al., 2015]
G. Dong, X. Lin, M.N. Potenza.
Decreased functional connectivity in an executive control network is related to impaired executive function in internet gaming disorder.
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 57 (2015), pp. 76-85
[Espinoza et al., 2019]
F.A. Espinoza, N.E. Anderson, V.M. Vergara, C.L. Harenski, J. Decety, S. Rachakonda, et al.
Resting-state fMRI dynamic functional network connectivity and associations with psychopathy traits.
NeuroImage: Clinical, 24 (2019),
[Fan et al., 2016]
L. Fan, H. Li, J. Zhuo, Y. Zhang, J. Wang, L. Chen, et al.
The human brainnetome atlas: A new brain atlas based on connectional architecture.
Cerebral Cortex, 26 (2016), pp. 3508-3526
[Favaretto et al., 2022]
C. Favaretto, M. Allegra, G. Deco, N.V. Metcalf, J.C. Griffis, G.L. Shulman, et al.
Subcortical-cortical dynamical states of the human brain and their breakdown in stroke.
Nature Communications, 13 (2022), pp. 5069
[Finc et al., 2020]
K. Finc, K. Bonna, X. He, D.M. Lydon-Staley, S. Kühn, W. Duch, et al.
Dynamic reconfiguration of functional brain networks during working memory training.
Nature Communications, 11 (2020), pp. 2435
[Freinhofer et al., 2020]
D. Freinhofer, P. Schwartenbeck, N. Thon, T. Eigenberger, W. Aichhorn, M. Lenger, et al.
Deficient decision making in pathological gamblers correlates with gray matter volume in medial orbitofrontal cortex.
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11 (2020), pp. 109
[Friston et al., 1996]
K.J. Friston, S. Williams, R. Howard, R.S.J. Frackowiak, R. Turner.
Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series: Movement artifacts in fMRI.
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 35 (1996), pp. 346-355
[Fusar-Poli et al., 2019]
P. Fusar-Poli, M. Solmi, N. Brondino, C. Davies, C. Chae, P. Politi, et al.
Transdiagnostic psychiatry: A systematic review.
World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA), 18 (2019), pp. 192-207
[Genauck et al., 2020]
A. Genauck, M. Andrejevic, K. Brehm, C. Matthis, A. Heinz, A. Weinreich, et al.
Cue-induced effects on decision-making distinguish subjects with gambling disorder from healthy controls.
Addiction Biology, 25 (2020), pp. e12841
[Genauck et al., 2021]
A. Genauck, C. Matthis, M. Andrejevic, L. Ballon, F. Chiarello, K. Duecker, et al.
Neural correlates of cue-induced changes in decision-making distinguish subjects with gambling disorder from healthy controls.
Addiction Biology, 26 (2021), pp. e12951
[Goudriaan et al., 2010]
A.E. Goudriaan, M.B. De Ruiter, W. Van Den Brink, J. Oosterlaan, D.J Veltman.
Brain activation patterns associated with cue reactivity and craving in abstinent problem gamblers, heavy smokers and healthy controls: An fMRI study: Neuroimaging cue reactivity in problem gamblers and smokers.
Addiction Biology, 15 (2010), pp. 491-503
[Grant and Chamberlain, 2023]
J.E. Grant, S.R. Chamberlain.
Impaired cognitive flexibility across psychiatric disorders.
CNS Spectrums, 28 (2023), pp. 688-692
[He et al., 2024a]
M. He, J. Lu, K.K. Tong.
Development of the protective beliefs and behaviours scale (PBBS) of problematic gaming.
Behaviour & Information Technology, 43 (2024), pp. 1798-1815
[He et al., 2024b]
M. He, Y. Ren, Y. Liu, K.K. Tong.
The prospective effect of protective gaming beliefs and behaviors on problematic gaming, mental health, and well-being of video game players.
International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, (2024), pp. 1-11
[He et al., 2018]
X. He, D.S. Bassett, G. Chaitanya, M.R. Sperling, L. Kozlowski, J.I. Tracy.
Disrupted dynamic network reconfiguration of the language system in temporal lobe epilepsy.
Brain : a Journal of Neurology, 141 (2018), pp. 1375-1389
[Hewig et al., 2010]
J. Hewig, N. Kretschmer, R.H. Trippe, H. Hecht, M.G.H. Coles, C.B. Holroyd, et al.
Hypersensitivity to reward in problem gamblers.
Biological Psychiatry, 67 (2010), pp. 781-783
[Hohenfeld et al., 2018]
C. Hohenfeld, C.J. Werner, K. Reetz.
Resting-state connectivity in neurodegenerative disorders: Is there potential for an imaging biomarker?.
NeuroImage: Clinical, 18 (2018), pp. 849-870
[Howard et al., 2020]
J.D. Howard, R. Reynolds, D.E. Smith, J.L. Voss, G. Schoenbaum, T. Kahnt.
Targeted stimulation of human orbitofrontal networks disrupts outcome-guided behavior.
Current Biology, 30 (2020), pp. 490-498
[Hutchison et al., 2013]
R.M. Hutchison, T. Womelsdorf, E.A. Allen, P.A. Bandettini, V.D. Calhoun, M. Corbetta, et al.
Dynamic functional connectivity: Promise, issues, and interpretations.
NeuroImage, 80 (2013), pp. 360-378
[Ionescu, 2012]
T. Ionescu.
Exploring the nature of cognitive flexibility.
New Ideas in Psychology, 30 (2012), pp. 190-200
[Jin et al., 2019]
Y. Jin, M. Yu, Y Hu.
The controversies and tendency of Internet Gaming Disorder research.
Advances in Psychological Science, 27 (2019), pp. 83-95
[Jung et al., 2014]
M.H. Jung, J.-H. Kim, Y.-C. Shin, W.H. Jung, J.H. Jang, J.-S. Choi, et al.
Decreased connectivity of the default mode network in pathological gambling: A resting state functional MRI study.
Neuroscience Letters, 583 (2014), pp. 120-125
[Kapoor and Narayanan, 2023]
S. Kapoor, A. Narayanan.
Leakage and the reproducibility crisis in machine-learning-based science.
[Kim et al., 2017]
Y.-J. Kim, J.A. Lim, J.Y. Lee, S. Oh, S.N. Kim, D.J. Kim, et al.
Impulsivity and compulsivity in Internet gaming disorder: A comparison with obsessive–compulsive disorder and alcohol use disorder.
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6 (2017), pp. 545-553
[Ko et al., 2014]
C.-H. Ko, T.-J. Hsieh, C.-Y. Chen, C.-F. Yen, C.-S. Chen, J.-Y. Yen, et al.
Altered brain activation during response inhibition and error processing in subjects with Internet gaming disorder: A functional magnetic imaging study.
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 264 (2014), pp. 661-672
[Ko et al., 2009]
C.-H. Ko, G.-C. Liu, S. Hsiao, J.-Y. Yen, M.-J. Yang, W.-C. Lin, et al.
Brain activities associated with gaming urge of online gaming addiction.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43 (2009), pp. 739-747
[Koehler et al., 2013]
S. Koehler, S. Ovadia-Caro, E. van der Meer, A. Villringer, A. Heinz, N. Romanczuk-Seiferth, et al.
Increased functional connectivity between prefrontal cortex and reward system in pathological gambling.
[Kuss et al., 2017]
D.J. Kuss, M.D. Griffiths, H.M. Pontes.
Chaos and confusion in DSM-5 diagnosis of Internet Gaming Disorder: Issues, concerns, and recommendations for clarity in the field.
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6 (2017), pp. 103-109
[Lavoie and Main, 2019]
R.V. Lavoie, K.J. Main.
When losing money and time feels good: The paradoxical role of flow in gambling.
Journal of Gambling Issues, 41 (2019),
[Leppink et al., 2016]
E.W. Leppink, S.A. Redden, S.R. Chamberlain, J.E. Grant.
Cognitive flexibility correlates with gambling severity in young adults.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 81 (2016), pp. 9-15
[Lu et al., 2020]
F. Lu, P. Liu, H. Chen, M. Wang, S. Xu, Z. Yuan, et al.
More than just statics: Abnormal dynamic amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation in adolescent patients with pure conduct disorder.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 131 (2020), pp. 60-68
[Luijten et al., 2017]
M. Luijten, A.F. Schellekens, S. Kühn, M.W.J. Machielse, G. Sescousse.
Disruption of reward processing in addiction: An image-based meta-analysis of functional Magnetic resonance imaging studies.
JAMA Psychiatry, 74 (2017), pp. 387
[Lurie et al., 2020]
D.J. Lurie, D. Kessler, D.S. Bassett, R.F. Betzel, M. Breakspear, S. Kheilholz, et al.
Questions and controversies in the study of time-varying functional connectivity in resting fMRI.
Network Neuroscience, 4 (2020), pp. 30-69
[Marraudino et al., 2022]
M. Marraudino, B. Bonaldo, B. Vitiello, G.C. Bergui, G. Panzica.
Sexual differences in Internet gaming disorder (IGD): From psychological features to neuroanatomical networks.
Journal of Clinical Medicine, 11 (2022), pp. 1018
[Miedl et al., 2015]
S.F. Miedl, D. Wiswede, J. Marco-Pallarés, Z. Ye, T. Fehr, M. Herrmann, et al.
The neural basis of impulsive discounting in pathological gamblers.
Brain Imaging and Behavior, 9 (2015), pp. 887-898
[Mirzaei and Adeli, 2022]
G. Mirzaei, H. Adeli.
Machine learning techniques for diagnosis of alzheimer disease, mild cognitive disorder, and other types of dementia.
Biomedical Signal Processing and Control, 72 (2022),
[Moccia et al., 2017]
L. Moccia, M. Pettorruso, F. De Crescenzo, L. De Risio, L. di Nuzzo, G. Martinotti, et al.
Neural correlates of cognitive control in gambling disorder: A systematic review of fMRI studies.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 78 (2017), pp. 104-116
[Mucha et al., 2010]
P.J. Mucha, T. Richardson, K. Macon, M.A. Porter, J.-P. Onnela.
Community structure in time-dependent, multiscale, and multiplex networks.
Science (New York, N.Y.), 328 (2010), pp. 876-878
[Parhami et al., 2012]
I. Parhami, A. Siani, R.J. Rosenthal, S. Lin, M. Collard, T.W. Fong.
Sleep and gambling severity in a community sample of gamblers.
Journal of Addictive Diseases, 31 (2012), pp. 67-79
[Pedersen et al., 2018]
M. Pedersen, A. Zalesky, A. Omidvarnia, G.D. Jackson.
Multilayer network switching rate predicts brain performance.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115 (2018), pp. 13376-13381
[Perandrés-Gómez et al., 2021]
A. Perandrés-Gómez, J.F. Navas, T. Van Timmeren, J.C. Perales.
Decision-making (in)flexibility in gambling disorder.
Addictive Behaviors, 112 (2021),
[Podlogar and Podlesek, 2022]
N. Podlogar, A. Podlesek.
Comparison of mental rotation ability, attentional capacity and cognitive flexibility in action video gamers and non-gamers.
Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 16 (2022),
[Power et al., 2012]
J.D. Power, K.A. Barnes, A.Z. Snyder, B.L. Schlaggar, S.E. Petersen.
Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion.
NeuroImage, 59 (2012), pp. 2142-2154
[Quaglieri et al., 2020]
A. Quaglieri, E. Mari, M. Boccia, L. Piccardi, C. Guariglia, A.M. Giannini.
Brain network underlying executive functions in gambling and alcohol use disorders: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of fMRI studies.
Brain Sciences, 10 (2020), pp. 353
[Raiha et al., 2020]
S. Raiha, G. Yang, L. Wang, W. Dai, H. Wu, G. Meng, et al.
Altered reward processing system in internet gaming disorder.
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11 (2020),
[Raimo et al., 2021]
S. Raimo, M. Cropano, L. Trojano, G. Santangelo.
The neural basis of gambling disorder: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 120 (2021), pp. 279-302
[Rolls, 2004]
E.T. Rolls.
The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex.
Brain and Cognition, 55 (2004), pp. 11-29
[Russell et al., 2022]
A.M.T. Russell, M. Browne, N. Hing, M. Rockloff, P. Newall.
Are any samples representative or unbiased? Reply to Pickering and Blaszczynski.
International Gambling Studies, 22 (2022), pp. 102-113
[Ryu et al., 2021]
K. Ryu, Y. Kim, J. Kim, M. Woo.
False accusation of online games: Internet gaming can enhance the cognitive flexibility of adolescents.
Asian Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1 (2021), pp. 116-121
[Schluter and Hodgins, 2021]
M.G. Schluter, D.C. Hodgins.
Reward-related decision-making in current and past disordered gambling: Implications for impulsive choice and risk preference in the maintenance of gambling disorder.
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 15 (2021),
[Smitha et al., 2017]
K.A. Smitha, R., K. Akhil, K.M. Arun, P.G. Rajesh, B. Thomas, T.R. Kapilamoorthy, et al.
Resting state fMRI: A review on methods in resting state connectivity analysis and resting state networks.
The Neuroradiology Journal, 30 (2017), pp. 305-317
[Stavropoulos et al., 2023]
V. Stavropoulos, D. Zarate, M. Prokofieva, N. Van De Berg, L. Karimi, A. Gorman Alesi, et al.
Deep learning(s) in gaming disorder through the user-avatar bond: A longitudinal study using machine learning.
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 12 (2023), pp. 878-894
[Subramaniam et al., 2015]
M. Subramaniam, P. Wang, P. Soh, J.A. Vaingankar, S.A. Chong, C.J. Browning, et al.
Prevalence and determinants of gambling disorder among older adults: A systematic review.
Addictive Behaviors, 41 (2015), pp. 199-209
[Sun et al., 2022]
F. Sun, Z. Liu, J. Yang, Z. Fan, C. Xi, P. Cheng, et al.
Shared and distinct patterns of dynamical degree centrality in bipolar disorder across different mood states.
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13 (2022),
[Tang et al., 2022]
J. Tang, Y. Xia, N. Liu, L. Li, P. Zou, P. Zhu, et al.
Growth hormone deficiency interferes with dynamic brain networks in short children.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 142 (2022),
[Thomas Yeo et al., 2011]
B.T. Thomas Yeo, F.M. Krienen, J. Sepulcre, M.R. Sabuncu, D. Lashkari, M. Hollinshead, et al.
The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 106 (2011), pp. 1125-1165
[Tong et al., 2020]
K.K. Tong, J.H. Chen, A.M.S. Wu.
Validation and application of the positive play scale adapted for Chinese gamblers: Its relation to disordered gambling and gambling attitudes.
Frontiers in Psychology, 11 (2020), pp. 263
[Treder, 2020]
M.S. Treder.
MVPA-light: A classification and regression toolbox for multi-dimensional data.
Frontiers in Neuroscience, 14 (2020), pp. 289
[Tschernegg et al., 2013]
M. Tschernegg, J.S. Crone, T. Eigenberger, P. Schwartenbeck, M. Fauth-Bühler, T. Lemènager, et al.
Abnormalities of functional brain networks in pathological gambling: A graph-theoretical approach.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7 (2013),
[van Holst et al., 2012]
R.J. van Holst, M. van Holstein, W. van den Brink, D.J. Veltman, A.E. Goudriaan.
Response inhibition during cue reactivity in problem gamblers: An fMRI study.
[Van Rooij and Kardefelt-Winther, 2017]
A.J. Van Rooij, D. Kardefelt-Winther.
Lost in the chaos: Flawed literature should not generate new disorders: Commentary on: Chaos and confusion in DSM-5 diagnosis of Internet Gaming Disorder: Issues, concerns, and recommendations for clarity in the field (Kuss et al.).
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6 (2017), pp. 128-132
[Van Timmeren et al., 2018]
T. Van Timmeren, J.G. Daams, R.J. Van Holst, A.E. Goudriaan.
Compulsivity-related neurocognitive performance deficits in gambling disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 84 (2018), pp. 204-217
[Wang et al., 2022]
Z.-L. Wang, M.N. Potenza, K.-R. Song, X.-Y. Fang, L. Liu, S.-S. Ma, et al.
Neural classification of Internet gaming disorder and prediction of treatment response using a cue-reactivity fMRI task in young men.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 145 (2022), pp. 309-316
[Weinberg, 2013]
D. Weinberg.
Post-humanism, addiction and the loss of self-control: Reflections on the missing core in addiction science.
International Journal of Drug Policy, 24 (2013), pp. 173-181
[Wiehler and Peters, 2015]
A. Wiehler, J. Peters.
Reward-based decision making in pathological gambling: The roles of risk and delay.
Neuroscience Research, 90 (2015), pp. 3-14
[Wong et al., 2020]
H.Y. Wong, H.Y. Mo, M.N. Potenza, M.N.M. Chan, W.M. Lau, T.K. Chui, et al.
Relationships between severity of internet gaming disorder, severity of problematic social media use, sleep quality and psychological distress.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (2020), pp. 1879
[Wood et al., 2017]
R.T.A. Wood, M.J.A. Wohl, N. Tabri, K. Philander.
Measuring responsible gambling amongst players: Development of the positive play scale.
Frontiers in Psychology, 8 (2017),
[World Health Organization 2022]
World Health Organization. (2022). WHO releases new International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11). https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en.
[Yalcinbas et al., 2021]
E.A. Yalcinbas, C. Cazares, C.M. Gremel.
Call for a more balanced approach to understanding orbital frontal cortex function.
Behavioral Neuroscience, 135 (2021), pp. 255-266
[Yan et al., 2016]
C.-G. Yan, X.-D. Wang, X.-N. Zuo, Y.-F. Zang.
DPABI: Data processing & analysis for (resting-state) brain imaging.
Neuroinformatics, 14 (2016), pp. 339-351
[Yan et al., 2017]
C.-G. Yan, Z. Yang, S.J. Colcombe, X.-N. Zuo, M.P. Milham.
Concordance among indices of intrinsic brain function: Insights from inter-individual variation and temporal dynamics.
Science Bulletin, 62 (2017), pp. 1572-1584
[Yan et al., 2021]
H. Yan, Q. Li, K. Yu, G. Zhao.
Large-scale network dysfunction in youths with internet gaming disorder: A meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity studies.
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 109 (2021),
[Yang et al., 2022]
F. Yang, X. Jiang, F. Yue, L. Wang, H. Boecker, Y. Han, et al.
Exploring dynamic functional connectivity alterations in the preclinical stage of Alzheimer's disease: An exploratory study from SILCODE.
Journal of Neural Engineering, 19 (2022),
[Yao et al., 2017]
Y.-W. Yao, L. Liu, S.-S. Ma, X.-H. Shi, N. Zhou, J.-T. Zhang, et al.
Functional and structural neural alterations in Internet gaming disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 83 (2017), pp. 313-324
[Zeng et al., 2022]
N. Zeng, M. Wang, H. Dong, X. Du, G.-H. Dong.
Altered dynamic interactions within frontostriatal circuits reflect disturbed craving processing in Internet gaming disorder.
CNS Spectrums, 27 (2022), pp. 109-117
[Zhang et al., 2021]
M. Zhang, X. Gao, Z. Yang, M. Wen, H. Huang, R. Zheng, et al.
Shared gray matter alterations in subtypes of addiction: A voxel-wise meta-analysis.
Psychopharmacology, 238 (2021), pp. 2365-2379
[Zheng et al., 2019]
H. Zheng, Y. Hu, Z. Wang, M. Wang, X. Du, G. Dong.
Meta-analyses of the functional neural alterations in subjects with internet gaming disorder: Similarities and differences across different paradigms.
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 94 (2019),
[Zhou et al., 2022]
H. Zhou, J.H. Chen, M.M. Spada, K.K. Tong, L. Dang, A.M.S. Wu.
Metacognitions about gambling among Chinese gamblers: Translation, validation, and application to understanding gambling disorder and responsible gambling.
International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, (2022),
[Zhou et al., 2023]
H. Zhou, Y. He, Z. Yuan, Y. Zhou, J. Yin, R. Chark, et al.
Altered hierarchical organization between empathy and gambling networks in disordered gamblers.
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 14 (2023),
[Zhou and Wu, 2024]
H. Zhou, A.M.S. Wu.
The protective effects of cognitive empathy and emotional empathy on gambling disorder are mediated by risk aversion and responsible gambling attitude.
BMC Psychiatry, 24 (2024), pp. 63
[Zhu et al., 2023]
L. Zhu, Y. Zhu, S. Li, Y. Jiang, X. Mei, Y. Wang, et al.
Association of Internet gaming disorder with impulsivity: Role of risk preferences.
BMC Psychiatry, 23 (2023), pp. 754
[Zou et al., 2008]
Q.-H. Zou, C.-Z. Zhu, Y. Yang, X.-N. Zuo, X.-Y. Long, Q.-J. Cao, et al.
An improved approach to detection of amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) for resting-state fMRI: Fractional ALFF.
Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 172 (2008), pp. 137-141
[Zuo et al., 2012]
X.-N. Zuo, R. Ehmke, M. Mennes, D. Imperati, F.X. Castellanos, O. Sporns, et al.
Network centrality in the human functional connectome.
Cerebral Cortex, 22 (2012), pp. 1862-1875

Co-first author.

Copyright © 2025. The Authors
Descargar PDF
Opciones de artículo