Elsevier

Neuroscience Letters

Volume 535, 22 February 2013, Pages 35-39
Neuroscience Letters

Small-world networks in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and first-episode schizophrenia during a working memory task

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.11.051Get rights and content

Abstract

Disturbances of functional interaction between different brain regions have been hypothesized to be the major pathophysiological mechanism underlying the cognitive deficits of schizophrenia. We investigated the small-world functional networks in individuals at ultra-high risk (UHR) for psychosis, first-episode schizophrenia (FESPR) patients, and healthy controls. All participants underwent the electroencephalogram during a control task and a working memory (WM) task. Small-world properties of the theta band were reduced in FESPR relative to controls during the WM task. Small-worldness of the UHR during the WM task exhibited intermediate value between that of controls and FESPR. These results imply that the suboptimal organization of the brain network may play a pivotal role in the schizophrenia pathophysiology.

Highlights

► Small-world network during working memory was investigated using the EEG. ► Small-worldness is reduced in first-episode schizophrenia and intermediate in UHR. ► Suboptimal network integration is suggested in schizophrenia pathophysiology.

Introduction

Schizophrenia is a chronic mental disorder with impairments in multiple cognitive domains including the working memory (WM) [18], [29]. Underlying these cognitive impairments, disturbances in functional interaction between different brain regions have been hypothesized [5], [6], [11]. A method of graph theoretical analysis investigating the so-called “small-world network” may offer valuable information in exploring this hypothesis. Characterized by a high clustering coefficient (an index of local clustering), and a short path length (an index of global integration), the small-world network reflects an optimal network topology for functional segregation and integration to maximize information processing efficiency between brain regions [1], [45], [46], [51]. Using the electroencephalography (EEG), small-world properties were found to be decreased in schizophrenia patients compared to healthy subjects during a WM task [38], [40]. It has also been demonstrated that decreased small-worldness is associated with lower cognitive function in healthy subjects [20]. Together, it has been suggested that the suboptimal small-world network may play an important role underlying the cognitive impairments of schizophrenia [38], [40].

Evidence suggests that cognitive deficits may be present not only in the first episode of schizophrenia but also prior to the emergence of frank psychotic symptoms. The ‘putative’ prodromal, ultra-high-risk for psychosis (UHR), subjects show cognitive impairments such as WM deficits [8], [15], [16], [30], [32]. In search of underlying neural dynamics, previous studies of small-world networks during cognitive tasks were limited in that subjects were chronic, multi-episode schizophrenia patients [38], [40]. The only small-world network study reported of first-episode patients explored the network properties during the resting state [41]. Furthermore, small-world network architecture in UHR subjects has not yet been investigated. Studying the small-world topology associated with cognitive function in first episode schizophrenia (FESPR) and those at UHR may provide an important insight at the ongoing pathophysiological alterations of network integration.

In the present study, we investigated the small-world network properties in UHR subjects, FESPR patients, and healthy controls (HC) during a WM task. Based on previous reports [38], [40], we hypothesized that the small-worldness will also be reduced in FESPR during the WM task. Since neurocognitive impairments are reported to be present before the frank onset of psychotic symptoms, we also hypothesized that small-world properties will be altered in UHR subjects, but to a lesser degree compared to FESPR patients.

Section snippets

Participants

Twelve FESPR, 13 UHR, and 13 HC subjects participated. Inclusion of UHR subjects was based on the Criteria of Prodromal Symptoms from the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes (SIPS) [39], requiring that individuals meet at least one of the following three clinical criteria: (1) brief intermittent psychotic syndrome (n = 4), (2) attenuated positive prodromal syndrome (n = 12), or (3) genetic risk and deterioration syndrome (n = 2), in which the genetic risk is determined if the subject has a

Results

As shown in Table 1, there were no between-group differences in demographics and clinical characteristics except the global neurocognitive composite scores and positive symptom severity. FESPR demonstrated lower neurocognitive scores compared to HC (p = 0.026). There were no significant differences in neurocognitive scores of UHR with that of other groups (UHR vs. HC: p = 0.087; UHR vs. FESPR: p = 1.00). Behavioral performance did not differ among groups for both the control task (Chi square of

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to compare small-world properties of the brain network among FESPR, UHR and controls. Our findings revealed significant between-group differences in small-world properties during the WM task, but not the control task. FESPR displayed reduced small-world index compared to controls, while small-worldness of UHR subjects were intermediate between that of HC and FESPR during the WM task.

The present study extends previous reports of disrupted small-world

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by a grant of the Korea Healthcare technology R&D project, Ministry for Health, Welfare & Family Affairs, Republic of Korea (A090096-0911-0000100). It was also supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grants funded by the Korea Government, the Ministry of Education, Science & Technology (MEST) (2009-0086964 and 2010-0017662) and by WCU (World Class University) program through the NRF of Korea funded by the MEST (R32-2008-000-10218-0).

References (52)

  • S. Achard et al.

    Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks

    PLoS Computational Biology

    (2007)
  • S. Achard et al.

    A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • N.C. Andreasen

    Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS)

    (1983)
  • N.C. Andreasen

    Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS)

    (1983)
  • N.C. Andreasen et al.

    Defining the phenotype of schizophrenia: cognitive dysmetria and its neural mechanisms

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1999)
  • N.C. Andreasen et al.

    “Cognitive dysmetria” as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuitry?

    Schizophrenia Bulletin

    (1998)
  • A. Baddeley

    Working memory: looking back and looking forward

    Nature Reviews Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • N. Barrantes-Vidal et al.

    Working memory in siblings of schizophrenia patients

    Schizophrenia Research

    (2007)
  • D.S. Bassett et al.

    Small-world brain networks

    Neuroscientist

    (2006)
  • D.S. Bassett et al.

    Adaptive reconfiguration of fractal small-world human brain functional networks

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2006)
  • M. Breakspear et al.

    A disturbance of nonlinear interdependence in scalp EEG of subjects with first episode schizophrenia

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • W.J. Brewer et al.

    Generalized and specific cognitive performance in clinical high-risk cohorts: a review highlighting potential vulnerability markers for psychosis

    Schizophrenia Bulletin

    (2006)
  • M.R. Broome et al.

    Neural correlates of executive function and working memory in the ‘at-risk mental state’

    British Journal of Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • T.D. Cannon et al.

    Prediction of psychosis in youth at high clinical risk: a multisite longitudinal study in North America

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • T.D. Cannon et al.

    The inheritance of neuropsychological dysfunction in twins discordant for schizophrenia

    American Journal of Human Genetics

    (2000)
  • H.M. Conklin et al.

    Working memory functioning in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives: cognitive functioning shedding light on etiology

    Neuropsychologia

    (2005)
  • C. Daban et al.

    Cognitive dysfunctions in medicated and unmedicated patients with recent-onset schizophrenia

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (2005)
  • A. Danielyan et al.

    Neurological disorders in schizophrenia

    The Psychiatric Clinics of North America

    (2009)
  • A. Delorme et al.

    EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis

    Journal of Neuroscience Methods

    (2004)
  • L. Douw et al.

    Cognition is related to resting-state small-world network topology: an magnetoencephalographic study

    Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • M.B. First et al.

    Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: Non-Patients Edition (SCID-I/PS)

    (1996)
  • M.B. First et al.

    Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: Patient Edition (SCID-I/P)

    (1996)
  • P. Fusar-Poli et al.

    Spatial working memory in individuals at high risk for psychosis: longitudinal fMRI study

    Schizophrenia Research

    (2010)
  • C.E. Ginestet et al.

    Statistical parametric network analysis of functional connectivity dynamics during a working memory task

    Neuroimage

    (2011)
  • M.F. Green

    Cognitive impairment and functional outcome in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder

    Journal of Clinical Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • M.E. Hasselmo

    What is the function of hippocampal theta rhythm? – linking behavioral data to phasic properties of field potential and unit recording data

    Hippocampus

    (2005)
  • Cited by (0)

    1

    These authors contributed equally to this study as first authors.

    View full text