Overt and relational aggression and victimization: Multiple perspectives within the school setting

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2007.05.003Get rights and content

Abstract

The current study involved a comprehensive comparative examination of overt and relational aggression and victimization across multiple perspectives in the school setting (peers, teachers, observers in the lunchroom, self-report). Patterns of results involving sociometic status, ethnicity and gender were explored among 4th graders, with particular emphasis on girls. Controversial and rejected children were perceived as higher on both forms of aggression than other status groups, but only rejected children were reported as victims. Both European American and African American girls showed a greater tendency toward relational aggression and victimization than overt aggression or victimization. Results indicated negative outcomes associated with both relational and overt victimization and especially overt aggression for the target girl sample. Poorer adjustment and a socially unskillful behavioral profile were found to be associated with these three behaviors. However, relational aggression did not evidence a similar negative relation to adjustment nor was it related to many of the behaviors examined in the current study. Implications of these results are discussed.

Introduction

For many years, studies of children's aggression and victimization focused on overt, confrontational behaviors like physical and verbal assault, threats, and insults. The vast literature that has been amassed confirms that for many children, overt aggression (see Coie & Dodge, 1998) and victimization (Juvonen & Graham, 2001) are recurring and stable problems that place children at risk for a host of adjustment difficulties, both internalizing and externalizing. More recently, conceptualizations of aggression and victimization have been broadened to include more “relational” forms of aggression such as negative gossip, ostracism, or manipulation of social relationships. Whether these forms of aggression are referred to as indirect, social, or relational aggression, all capture to some degree behaviors that are intended to harm another's reputation, social relationships, or feelings of inclusion by the peer group. Ranging from direct and unambiguous exclusion to indirect and even disguised attacks (e.g., negative gossip in the guise of helpful advice), relational forms of aggression are characterized by an intent to do social harm, using relationships to inflict the damage.

The inclusion of relational aggression in our conceptualization of harm has also broadened the range of children identified as potential targets of intervention, and most of those identified in this casting of a wider net are girls. In fact, researchers have found that as many as 60% of girl aggressors (Henington, Hughes, Cavell, & Thompson, 1998) and 71.4% of girl victims (Crick & Nelson, 2002) would be overlooked if intervention programs excluded relational forms of harm from their identification of perpetrators and victims (corresponding rates for boys were only 7% of aggressors and 21.1% of victims). The predominant instigators and targets of physical aggression are boys (Coie & Dodge, 1998), so it is not surprising that a literature historically focused on physical aggression would place so little emphasis on girls. However, over the past decade, growing interest in girls' aggression has generated much research activity (Pepler et al., 2005, Putallaz and Bierman, 2004), including a proliferation of studies on relational aggression and its victims. But the topic is still in its infancy and many questions remain.

The current study aims to further the development of this emerging literature with a comprehensive comparative examination of aggression and victimization in the school setting, with a particular emphasis on girls. We first collected peer nominations from a large and diverse sample of elementary aged children (boys and girls) to validate and expand upon previous research on gender, ethnicity, and sociometric status differences in both overt and relational forms of aggression and victimization. To address the paucity of data on girls, we then selected a smaller sub-sample of African American and European American girls for a more in-depth study of girls' relational aggression and victimization as compared to more overt forms. Using a multi-method, multi-informant approach, the objectives of the current study were to: (1) explore the role of ethnicity and sociometric status in aggression and victimization; (2) address unresolved questions in the adjustment correlates of aggression and victimization, particularly relational forms; (3) provide a broader and more complete behavioral profile of aggressors and their victims.

Section snippets

Gender

Because interest in girls' aggression evolved in tandem with an increased focus on aggression's more relational forms, the implication might be that most relational aggressors are females, and, in fact, that stereotype predominates in the popular press. However, empirical evidence is less compelling; although some studies find girls more relationally aggressive than boys, others report no gender differences or even higher rates of relational aggression among boys than girls (for a review, see

Consistency across multiple informants

Although past research indicates that cross-informant agreement varies as a function of the child's age, gender, and type of aggression involved (Hawker and Boulton, 2000, McEvoy et al., 2003, McNeilly-Choque et al., 1996, Tomada and Schneider, 1997), generally peer reports and teacher reports of aggression are more highly correlated than either is with self-report (e.g., Cairns et al., 1989, Xie et al., 2002). In contrast, low to moderate correlations among teacher, peer, and self-reports of

Participants

The current research involves data from a comprehensive study of two successive cohorts of 4th grade public school children in a midsized southeastern city. Sociometric nominations were obtained from a total of 1397 students on 913 boys and 915 girls in the 78 4th-grade classrooms of 13 public schools. Five of the schools were predominantly African American, three were predominantly European American, and five were racially balanced. Participation rates within schools ranged from 62% to 96%,

Teacher measure

The teachers of all target girls completed questionnaires in the spring evaluating the girls on a variety of dimensions. The measure was adapted from the Teacher Checklist of Social Behavior (Coie, Terry, Underwood, & Dodge, 1992) and consists of 82 items comprising 15 scales (i.e., overt aggression, relational aggression, overt victimization, relational victimization, hyperactivity, disruption, social avoidance, fear of negative evaluation, depression, empathy, leadership, inclusiveness, entry

Consistency of aggression and victimization ratings

The first sets of analyses concerned the inter-rater consistency of the assessments of aggression and victimization within the target sample of girls. The correlations reported in Table 1 permit an examination of the agreement across reporters on the two types of aggression and victimization within our target sample. Peers and teachers agreed on who they perceived to be overt and relational aggressors as well as victims of both overt aggression and relational aggression. However, the

Gender, ethnicity, and sociometric status differences in the larger sample

In the larger sociometric data set, which included behavioral nominations of both boys and girls, it was possible to examine the relationships between gender, ethnicity and sociometric status as predictors of aggression and victimization. A repeated measures Multivariate Analysis of Variance (RM-MANOVA) revealed significant main effects for all subject variables [Gender: F(4,1677) = 36.70, p < .001; Ethnicity: F(4,1677) = 16.30, p < .001; Status: F(12,4437) = 3.24, p < .01]. The RM-MANOVA also revealed

Role of gender, ethnicity, and sociometric status in aggression and victimization

A first purpose of the current study was to investigate the role of gender, ethnicity and sociometric status in overt and relational aggression and victimization. Regarding gender, our results conform to predictions. Boys were perceived by peers as more overtly aggressive than girls, whereas there were no gender differences in the use of relational aggression. In terms of victimization, boys were slightly more likely than girls to be seen by peers as victims of overt aggression, whereas girls

Summary and future directions

The current study involved a comprehensive examination of overt and relational aggression and victimization across multiple perspectives in the school setting. Controversial and rejected boys and girls were perceived as higher on both forms of aggression than other status groups, but only rejected children were reported as victims. Both European and African American girls showed a greater tendency toward relational aggression and victimization than overt aggression or victimization. As

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by grant MH52843-05 from the National Institute of Mental Health. Portions of this research were presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, New Mexico, April, 1999. The authors wish to acknowledge the statistical assistance provided by Blair Sheppard. We are grateful to the children and teachers who participated in this research and to the staff of the Social Development Lab for their contributions to data coding.

References (51)

  • N.R. Crick et al.

    Relational and overt forms of peer victimization: A multi-informant approach

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1998)
  • N.R. Crick et al.

    Relational aggression, gender, and social–psychological adjustment

    Child Development

    (1995)
  • N.R. Crick et al.

    Children's treatment by peers: Victims of overt and relational aggression

    Development and Psychopathology

    (1996)
  • N.R. Crick et al.

    Relationally and physically aggressive children's intent attributions and feelings of distress for relational and instrumental peer provocations

    Child Development

    (2002)
  • N.R. Crick et al.

    Relational and physical victimization within friendships: Nobody told me there'd be friends like these

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2002)
  • C. Cullerton-Sen et al.

    Understanding the effects of physical and relational victimization: The utility of multiple perspectives in predicting social–emotional adjustment

    School Psychology Review

    (2005)
  • C.F. David et al.

    Do positive self-perceptions have a “dark side?” Examination of the link between perceptual bias and aggression

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2000)
  • J.M. Gottman et al.

    Speculations about social and affective development: Friendship and acquaintanceship through adolescence

  • L.D. Hanish et al.

    The roles of ethnicity and school context in predicting children's victimization by peers

    American Journal of Community Psychology

    (2000)
  • D.S.J. Hawker et al.

    Twenty years' research on peer victimization and psychosocial maladjustment: A meta-analytic review of cross-sectional studies

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • S.A. Hill et al.

    Parenting in Black and White families: The interaction of gender with race and class

    Gender & Society

    (1999)
  • Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four-factor index of social status. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Sociology, Yale...
  • C.A. Hudley

    Comparing teacher and peer perceptions of aggression: An ecological approach

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1993)
  • J. Juvonen et al.

    Peer harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized

    (2001)
  • A. Kaukiainen et al.

    The relationships between social intelligence, empathy, and three types of aggression

    Aggressive Behavior

    (1999)
  • Cited by (212)

    • Co-rumination with friends exacerbates association between peer victimization and adjustment in adolescence

      2022, Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Generally, the sizes of effects are larger when informants are the same than when they were different (Hawker & Boulton, 2000). It has been found that self-reported peer victimization was more strongly correlated than peer-reported peer victimization with self-reported psychological maladjustment, and peer-reported victimization was more strongly correlated than self-reported victimization with peer-reported psychological maladjustment (Crick & Bigbee, 1998; Hawker & Boulton, 2000; Putallaz et al., 2007). In the present study, peer-reported peer victimization and self-reported internalizing problems have been taken into consideration, which might contribute to the nonsignificant association between peer victimization and depressive symptoms and the weak association between peer victimization and anxiety.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text