Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 100, Issue 1, May 2006, Pages 100-130
Cognition

Are we “experienced listeners”? A review of the musical capacities that do not depend on formal musical training

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.007Get rights and content

Abstract

The present paper reviews a set of studies designed to investigate different aspects of the capacity for processing Western music. This includes perceiving the relationships between a theme and its variations, perceiving musical tensions and relaxations, generating musical expectancies, integrating local structures in large-scale structures, learning new compositional systems and responding to music in an emotional (affective) way. The main focus of these studies was to evaluate the influence of intensive musical training on these capacities. The overall set of data highlights that some musical capacities are acquired through exposure to music without the help of explicit training. These capacities reach such a degree of sophistication that they enable untrained listeners to respond to music as “musically experienced listeners” do.

Introduction

The origins of musical competence have been largely debated and even today it remains an open question (Deliège and Sloboda, 1997, Peretz and Zatorre, 2003, Wallin et al., 1999). In this paper, we will argue that studying the influence of intensive musical training on the perception of music contributes to highlight the very nature of human capacity for processing and understanding music. If this capacity is rooted in innate predispositions (Trehub, 2003) that have been selected through evolution for adaptation (social bonding, for example, Cross, 2003), musical competence should be largely shared in the general population. Moreover, given the richness of musical stimulations in everyday life, and given the remarkable ability of human beings to internalize regularities of the auditory environment through implicit learning processes (Saffran et al., 1996, Saffran et al., 1999, Saffran et al., 1996), it is likely that this initial predisposition could become considerably sophisticated in adulthood. Along this line, listeners without musical training but with sufficient exposure to a given musical idiom may be viewed as “experienced listeners” who use the same principles as musically trained listeners when listening to music and structuring what they hear, “but in a more limited way” (Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983, p. 3). How this learning takes place in the general public remains an open issue, which we will return to in the last section of the paper.

A quite opposite approach to the implicit learning hypothesis stipulates that musical competence is mostly determined by an intensive musical training and remains rather rough in untrained listeners (see Levinson, 1997 for a debate; Wolpert, 1990, Wolpert, 2000). Some authors have argued that musical abilities develop naturally up to 10 years but do not longer evolve without explicit musical training (Francès, Zenatti, & Imberty, 1979). Verbal reports provided after the experiments by our participants (both musically trained or untrained) usually support this view. Explicit training is supposed to have a deep impact on both the cognitive and neural architectures, notably when training starts early in life (Pantev & Lütkenhöner, 2000). Recent studies showing that musicians’ and nonmusicians’ brains differ from both anatomical and functional ways argue along this line (Elbert et al., 1995, Gaser and Schlaug, 2003, Pantev et al., 2003, Schlaug, 2001). In a recent study, Seung, Kyong, Woo, Lee, and Lee (2005) provided evidence that comparable musical tasks performed with the same stimuli differently activated the brain on prior musical training of participants. For some authors, an important issue is thus to understand whether these brain differences result from training or correspond to genetic differences that predispose some individuals to become musicians (Schneider et al., 2002, Thompson et al., 2001). In cognitive psychology, the influence of intensive training on the processing capacities of experts has been well established in several fields, such as chess (Chase & Simon, 1973), mathematics (Schoenfeld & Herrmann, 1982), physics (Chi, Feltovich, & Glaser, 1981) or more recently wine expertise (Chollet and Valentin, 2000, Hughson and Boakes, 2002). Training lead experts to develop skills, processing strategies and declarative knowledge that are not found in novices. These skills influence the way experts attend to and encode stimuli from their field of expertise by comparison to novices. The effect of expertise stands similarly in music. Musical training is very demanding since it requires several hours of training per week and several hours of practice per day for a very long period (from 10 to 15 years in French conservatories). During this period musicians learn specific perceptual skills in ear training, and music analysis and acquire a lot of declarative knowledge about the structure of Western music. Moreover, they develop motor skills to be able to play music. All of these skills are likely to deeply influence the way they process musical stimuli. For example, the motor skills that are activated during music listening influences the processing of musical stimuli (Haueisen and Knösche, 2001, Janata and Grafton, 2003). In a related vein, the possibility to use the linguistic labels of musical events enable musically trained listeners to encode musical structures in a more relevant way than “musically illiterate persons” can (Francès, 1958). As a consequence, it is reasonable to expect important differences between musically trained and untrained listeners (see Levinson, 1997 for a debate; Wolpert, 1990, Wolpert, 2000).

Delineating the musical abilities that are specifically linked to an intensive and formal training from those that emerge through mere exposure to music is a key issue for music cognition. Nonmusicians do not learn a formal system with which they can describe and think about musical structures but they have a considerable amount of experience with music: they hear music every day of their lives, they all sung as children and sung in school, they have moved and danced to musical rhythms, and most of them have attended concerts. How sophisticated are the emergent abilities to process music that result from this exposure when compared to those caused by an intensive formal musical training? Given the huge difference in training, finding differences between musically trained and untrained listeners would not be really surprising. However, showing that some elaborated musical capacities do not strongly differ as a function of musical expertise, would be critical to further our understanding of the human aptitude for music. The aim of the present paper is to survey several findings collected in our laboratory on that issue. The initial goal of the research program was to identify the type of structures untrained listeners were unable to process by contrast to trained listeners.1 This issue was addressed by considering several aspects of Western music perception, ranging from melodic and harmonic processing to emotional responses to music. Before explaining these studies in detail, two methodological considerations deserve attention.

Section snippets

Which methods can be used to compare musically trained and untrained participants?

Numerous examples indicate that the experimental task, which participants are required to complete, considerably matters when probing the competence of musically untrained listeners. The initial experiments of the categorical perception of musical intervals indicated that nonmusicians were unable to categorize minor versus major intervals (Burns, 1982). A change in task demonstrated that they actually did categorize musical intervals in a similar way as musically trained listeners did (Smith,

Processing underlying musical structures

The goal of the first set of studies was to investigate the ability of listeners to process the musical progression that underlies the musical surface. The distinction between surface (i.e., the acoustical feature linked to pitch, loudness, timbre, and so forth) and underlying musical structures is crucial to account for music competence (Lerdahl and Jackendoff, 1983, Schenker, 1935, Sloboda, 1985). If musical pieces were no more than suites of charming sounding stimuli, there would be a

Musical expectancies

The second set of studies addresses another aspect of music cognition, fundamentally related to the previous ones, but dealing more directly with musical expectancies. Studying the formation of musical expectancies offers an interesting opportunity to investigate the ability to process music at least for two reasons. First, perceptual anticipatory processes are of considerable importance in music since emotional responses are likely to be determined by how the composer fulfills or not the

Processing large-scale structures

The preceding studies investigated the ability of listeners to process structures of Western music on short time spans, usually no longer than 30 s. To what extent can the reduced nature of these musical stimuli explain the weak effect of musical training? Several other studies have been conducted with longer pieces stemming from the existing musical repertoire (Tillmann and Bigand, 1998, Tillmann et al., 1998). In these studies, which involved explicit tasks, musicians performed better than

Learning new musical idioms

A further important aspect of the musical capacity of the human brain rests on the ability of listeners to assimilate new musical idioms. To what extent may an intensive musical training help to learn new musical idioms? Musically experienced listeners should not only be able to process subtle musical structures of their familiar musical idiom, they are also expected to better internalize and comprehend new musical idioms coming either from other human cultures, or from new compositional

Emotional responses to music

Finally, the most critical aspect of the human capacity for music resides in the ability to emotionally (or affectively) respond to musical stimuli. Musical activities would probably have disappeared from all human societies if listeners were unable to confer (or perceive) an affective value to musical stimuli. Being able to apprehend the emotional quality of musical stimuli is so important that it might be at the heart of all musical abilities. How does musical training influence emotional or

Conclusion

This set of studies highlights several cognitive characteristics of the human capacity to perceive and process music: this capacity rests on fast-acting and irrepressible processes that enable us to extract subtle musical structures from short musical pieces, but that are less efficient when integrating local patterns into large-scale structures (see Levinson, 1997 for a discussion). It seems that these processes can be adjusted relatively fast to new musical idioms, even highly complex ones.

References (121)

  • Barthes, R. (1985). “Music’s dody,” part II of the responsibility of forms: Critical essays on music, art, and...
  • M. Besson et al.

    An event-related potential (ERP) study of musical expectancy: Comparison of musicians with nonmusicians

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1995)
  • M. Besson et al.

    Brain waves associated with musical incongruities differ for musicians and non-musicians

    Neuroscience Letters

    (1994)
  • T.G. Bever et al.

    Cerebral dominance in musicians and nonmusicians

    Science

    (1974)
  • J.J. Bharucha et al.

    Reaction time and musical expectancy: Priming of chords

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1986)
  • J.J. Bharucha et al.

    Priming of chords: Spreading activation or overlapping frequency spectra?

    Perception & Psychophysics

    (1987)
  • E. Bigand

    Abstraction of two forms of underlying structure in a tonal melody

    Psychology of Music

    (1990)
  • E. Bigand

    The influence of implicit harmony, rhythm and musical training on the abstraction of “tension–relaxation schemas” in a tonal musical phrase

    Contemporary Music Review

    (1993)
  • E. Bigand

    Perceiving musical stability: The effect of tonal structure, rhythm, and musical expertise

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1997)
  • Bigand, E., D’Adamo, D. A., Poulin-Charronnat, B. (in revision). The implicit learning of twelve-tone...
  • E. Bigand et al.

    Effect of global structure and temporal organization on chord processing

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1999)
  • E. Bigand et al.

    Perceiving musical tension in long chord sequences

    Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung

    (1999)
  • E. Bigand et al.

    Perception of musical tension in short chord sequences: The influence of harmonic function, sensory dissonance, horizontal motion, and musical training

    Perception & Psychophysics

    (1996)
  • E. Bigand et al.

    Implicit learning of an artificial grammar of musical timbres

    Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition

    (1998)
  • E. Bigand et al.

    Context effects on melody recognition: A dynamic interpretation

    Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition

    (1996)
  • E. Bigand et al.

    Global context effects on musical expectancy

    Perception & Psychophysics

    (1997)
  • E. Bigand et al.

    Sensory versus cognitive components in harmonic priming

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (2003)
  • E. Bigand et al.

    The effect of harmonic context on phoneme monitoring in vocal music

    Cognition

    (2001)
  • E. Bigand et al.

    Repetition priming: Is music special?

    The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2005)
  • E. Bigand et al.

    Multidimensional scaling of emotional responses to music: The effect of musical expertise and of the duration of the excerpts

    Cognition and Emotion

    (2005)
  • S. Brandler et al.

    Differences in mental abilities between musicians and non-musicians

    Psychology of Music

    (2003)
  • E. Brattico et al.

    Context effects on pitch perception in musicians and nonmusicians: Evidence from event-related-potential recordings

    Music Perception

    (2002)
  • E.M. Burns

    Intervals, scales, and tuning

  • A.S. Chan et al.

    Music training improves verbal memory

    Nature

    (1998)
  • W.G. Chase et al.

    Perception in chess

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1973)
  • J.M. Cheek et al.

    Music training and mathematics achievement

    Adolescence

    (1999)
  • M.T.H. Chi et al.

    Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices

    Cognitive Science

    (1981)
  • Chollet, S., Valentin, D. (2000). Le degré d’expertise a-t-il une influence sur la perception olfactive? Quelques...
  • N. Cook

    The perception of large-scale tonal closure

    Music Perception

    (1987)
  • I. Cross

    Music as a biocultural phenomenon

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (2003)
  • L.L. Cuddy et al.

    Recovery of the tonal hierarchy: Some comparisons across age and levels of musical experience

    Perception & Psychophysics

    (1987)
  • L.L. Cuddy et al.

    Asymmetry of perceived key movement in chorale sequences: Converging evidence from a probe-tone analysis

    Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung

    (1992)
  • C. Deliège

    Les fondements de la musique tonale

    (1984)
  • I. Deliège et al.

    Perception and cognition of music

    (1997)
  • Z. Dienes et al.

    Can musical transformations be implicitly learned?

    Cognitive Science

    (2004)
  • Eitan, Z., Granot, R. Y. (2004, 15–18 April). Musical parameters and images of motion. Paper presented at the...
  • T. Elbert et al.

    Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players

    Science

    (1995)
  • Fraisse, P. (1967). Psychologie du temps [The psychology of time]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de...
  • R. Francès

    La perception de la musique

    (1958)
  • R. Francès et al.

    Le domaine musical

  • T. Fujioka et al.

    Musical training enhances automatic encoding of melodic contour and interval structure

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2004)
  • C. Gaser et al.

    Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians

    The Journal of Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • A.R. Halpern

    Memory for the absolute pitch of familiar songs

    Memory & Cognition

    (1989)
  • J. Haueisen et al.

    Involuntary motor activity in pianists evoked by music perception

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2001)
  • S. Hébert et al.

    Perceiving the tonal ending of tune excerpts: The roles of pre-existing representation and musical expertise

    Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1995)
  • A.L. Hughson et al.

    The knowing nose: The role of knowledge in wine expertise

    Food Quality and Preference

    (2002)
  • D. Huron et al.

    An improved model of tonality perception incorporating pitch salience and echo memory

    Psychomusicology

    (1993)
  • R. Jackendoff

    Musical parsing and musical affect

    Music Perception

    (1991)
  • P. Janata et al.

    Swinging in the brain: Shared neural substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • M. Karno et al.

    The effects of structural interventions in the first movement of Mozart’s Symphony in G Minor K. 550 on aesthetic preference

    Music Perception

    (1992)
  • Cited by (0)

    Thanks are due to Barbara Tillmann and Philippe Lalitte for helpful comments and suggestions on the manuscript. This research was supported by a grant from The International Foundation for Music Research.

    View full text