Elsevier

Appetite

Volume 125, 1 June 2018, Pages 210-216
Appetite

Sleep, food cravings and taste

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.02.013Get rights and content

Abstract

Objective

Taste is influenced by factors from our environment, psychology, and from our own physiological state. The objective of the study was to determine whether sleep influences our sense of taste or our cravings for food.

Method

57 healthy panelists, predominantly of college age, submitted to sleep tracking, and subsequently underwent a series of sensory tests, using basic prototypic tastants, as well as real foods. Panelists were also evaluated to quantify food cravings, using both the Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire, and the Control of Eating Questionnaire.

Results

Umami (p = 0.025, F = 5.301) and sour (p = 0.037, F = 4.591) taste were intensified in those rating sleepiness higher, while this group also reported higher implicit wanting for high fat sweet foods (p = 0.011, Wald chi-sq = 14.937). Craving for sweet or savory also associated with a number of measures of taste response to real foods.

Conclusions

Results imply that a lack of sleep may induce cravings for unhealthy foods, and that foods high in umami or sour taste may be experienced differently due to alterations in taste function. Results imply that feeding behavior may be influenced by a lack of sleep, acting at least partially through our sense of taste.

Introduction

Sleep functions as a restorative process of the brain, and is crucial for our health (Hobson, 1995). Behavioral sleep curtailment has become a major health concern for modern society: more people customarily are sacrificing their sleep, often for work or study (Hicks and Pellegrini, 1991, Beccuti and Pannain, 2011, Matricciani et al., 2012). Research has shown that sleep curtailment can increase the chance of developing metabolic disorders including diabetes and obesity (Appelhans et al., 2014, Di Milia et al., 2013). Those with sleep durations less than 6 h, or greater than 8 h have greater diabetes risk (Zizi et al., 2012), possibly due to sleep loss interfering with metabolic or endocrine functions. Sleep is a regulator of neuroendocrine function and glucose metabolism, with lack of sleep leading to negative health consequences such as altered insulin sensitivity, and an imbalance in leptin and ghrelin levels (Spiegel et al., 2004, Beccuti and Pannain, 2011; also see reviews by Van Cauter et al., 2007, Spiegel et al., 2009); factors also linked to taste function (Baquero and Gilbertson, 2011, Dando, 2010, Kawai et al., 2000, Shin et al., 2010). Various studies have investigated links between sleep and either cravings for, or intake of, foods. Increased sleep led to a decrease in both overall appetite and the desire for sweet or salty foods in a study of young adults (Tasali, Chapotot, Wroblewski, & Schoeller, 2014). Conversely, adolescents sleeping less than 8 h a day showed significantly increased consumption of calories from fat and carbohydrates (Weiss et al., 2010). Spiegel et al. (2004) demonstrated an enhanced appetite as well as increases approaching significance in reported desire for sweet, starchy and salty foods in those undergoing sleep restriction vs sleep extension. While the total weight of food consumed by panelists in the experiments of Cain, Filtness, Phillips, and Anderson (2015) was unchanged in a model of disturbed sleep (a simulated night shift), participants ate significantly more high-fat foods after sleep manipulation. When sleep deprived, panelists showed a greater desire for high calorie foods, with no change in wanting of low calorie items (Greer, Goldstein, & Walker, 2013), again highlighting a vulnerability for unhealthy eating behavior with sleep loss.

Sleep can be quantified using measures such as sleep duration, with sleep quality scores, or via instantaneous subjective ratings of sleepiness. Residential sleep studies or tracking devices are most effective in determining sleep duration, while sleep quality is often measured with scoring systems such as the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman, & Kupfer, 1989), with ratings of sleepiness most regularly quantified using the Stanford (Hoddes, Zarcone, Smythe, Phillips, & Dement, 1973) or Karolinska (Åkerstedt & Gillberg, 1990) sleepiness scales. In a large meta-analysis of several thousand participants, self-reported ratings of sleepiness showed the strongest relation to a panelist's cognitive performance, with sleep quality and sleep duration the next most effective respectively (Dewald, Meijer, Oort, Kerkhof, & Bögels, 2010).

This study sought to test whether a relationship exists between sleep patterns and taste function. A number of groups have reported no link between sleep and taste (Hogenkamp et al., 2013, Smith et al., 2016), however the majority of studies performed to date are limited to testing sweet and/or salty taste; the impact of sleep on umami, sourness, and bitterness remain particularly sparsely studied. Although taste response can predict eating habits (Keller et al., 2002, Noel et al., 2017), and taste is the primary driver of food choice (IFIC, 2017), other factors also contribute to the foods we select. Food cravings refer to an individual's urge to seek out and consume specific foods (Tiggemann & Kemps, 2005). Craving a food can also predict overconsumption of that food (Martin, O’Neil, Tollefson, Greenway, & White, 2008). Thus, as a secondary hypothesis, we sought to examine whether cravings for specific food categories would also associate with taste response to such foods.

Section snippets

Participants

Recruiting was conducted through the Cornell Sensory Evaluation Center mailing list, on Facebook, and via flyers posted on campus. All parts of the study were approved by Cornell University's Institutional Review Board for human participants. A small cash incentive was offered to each panelist for participation. All participants agreed to use the “Pillow” app (Neybox Interactive, Greece), and to provide us with their sleep data. The study took place in an exam week, where we expected more

Demographics

57 participants completed the study, of which 80% were female, 93% of participants were between ages of 18–34, with 5% 35–44 and 2% 65 or older (Table 1). The panel was 26% White, 9% Hispanic, 2% African American, and 56% Asian or Pacific Islanders, with 7% identifying as other or preferring not to identify. Both sleep quality, and measured sleep duration were collected via the sleep tracking app. Only 1 panelist reported to be dieting, and 12 reported having trouble sleeping, despite no

Discussion

On the whole (see Fig. 1, Fig. 2), it was evident that those reporting as more sleepy seem to rate taste as somewhat more intense than those reporting as not sleepy, particularly evident at high concentrations. Sleepiness was a significant factor in the perceived intensity of both umami and sour taste, with both rated as significantly higher with increasing sleepiness. Sleepier panelists also reported a higher implicit wanting for high fat sweet foods, perhaps leaving them vulnerable to

Conclusions

Sleep is regularly linked with metabolic health and diseases of consumption such as obesity and diabetes. Our study demonstrates that both taste response and the drive for certain foods associate with sleep status. These results indicate that feelings of sleepiness may encourage overconsumption of unhealthy foods, and thus provides support for links between obesity and sleep.

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Alina Stelick and members of the Dando lab for assistance with project execution.

References (45)

  • C. Noel et al.

    The effect of emotional state on taste perception

    Appetite

    (2015)
  • C.A. Noel et al.

    Participants with pharmacologically impaired taste function seek out more intense, higher calorie stimuli

    Appetite

    (2017)
  • S.L. Smith et al.

    Changes in taste preference and steps taken after sleep curtailment

    Physiology & Behavior

    (2016)
  • E. Tasali et al.

    The effects of extended bedtimes on sleep duration and food desire in overweight young adults: A home-based intervention

    Appetite

    (2014)
  • M. Tiggemann et al.

    The phenomenology of food cravings: The role of mental imagery

    Appetite

    (2005)
  • E. Van Cauter et al.

    Metabolic consequences of sleep and sleep loss

    Sleep Medicine

    (2008)
  • F. Zizi et al.

    Race/ethnicity, sleep duration, and diabetes mellitus: Analysis of the national health interview survey

    The American Journal of Medicine

    (2012)
  • T. Åkerstedt et al.

    Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual

    International Journal of Neuroscience

    (1990)
  • B.M. Appelhans et al.

    The home environment and childhood obesity in low-income households: Indirect effects via sleep duration and screen time

    BMC Public Health

    (2014)
  • A.F. Baquero et al.

    Insulin activates epithelial sodium channel (ENaC) via phosphoinositide 3-kinase in mammalian taste receptor cells

    American Journal of Physiology - cell Physiology

    (2011)
  • G. Beccuti et al.

    Sleep and obesity

    Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition and Metabolic Care

    (2011)
  • H. Cai et al.

    Altered lipid and salt taste responsivity in ghrelin and GOAT null mice

    PLoS One

    (2013)
  • Cited by (33)

    • Understanding the relationship between umami taste sensitivity and genetics, food-related behavior, and nutrition

      2023, Current Opinion in Food Science
      Citation Excerpt :

      Sleep and exercise have been studied to explore the relationships between these factors and umami taste sensitivity. Individuals who lacked sleep were more sensitive to umami [20]. Males have increased umami taste sensitivity and decreased umami preference after exercise [21].

    • Poor Sleep Quality Is Associated with Altered Taste Perception in Chinese Adults

      2021, Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics
      Citation Excerpt :

      Similarly, chronic sleep restriction (6.5 hours in bed per night) also led to increased consumption of sweetened food compared with healthy sleep duration (10 hours in bed per night) in 41 adolescents.45 Three cross-sectional studies (n < 58 for all) also observed altered taste perception among participants with short sleep duration.21-23 No significant association between snoring and altered taste perception was observed.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text