Elsevier

Behaviour Research and Therapy

Volume 36, Issue 9, 1 September 1998, Pages 899-913
Behaviour Research and Therapy

Relationships between worry, obsessive–compulsive symptoms and meta-cognitive beliefs

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00070-9Get rights and content

Abstract

The present study had two principal aims. First, the overlap between the revised Padua Inventory (PI-WSUR) a measure of obsessive–compulsive symptoms (Burns et al., 1996) and worry was assessed. Second, the relationship between meta-cognitive beliefs, proneness to pathological worry and obsessive–compulsive symptoms was explored when controlling for the interdependency of worry and obsessive–compulsive measures. The results indicate that whilst the PI-WSUR shows reduced overlap with the Penn State Worry Questionnaire, there are still problematic levels of overlap with a more content-based measure of worry. Results of multiple regression analyses demonstrated that specific sets of meta-cognitive beliefs were associated with worry proneness and obsessive–compulsive symptoms. The present data are consistent with recent formulations of generalized anxiety disorder and obsessive–compulsive disorder.

Introduction

Worry and obsessions are two types of intrusive cognitive phenomena associated with significant distress. They are characteristic features of particular emotional disorders such as generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). Worry and obsessions are also normally occurring phenomena (e.g. Rachman and de Silva, 1978; Salkovskis and Harrison, 1984; Wells and Morrison, 1994) and nonclinical samples can provide an adequate analogue for research on OCD (e.g. Burns et al., 1995).

Several self-report instruments have been developed to assess worry. These include the Anxious Thoughts Inventory (AnTI; Wells, 1994b), the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer et al., 1990) and the Worry Domains Questionnaire (WDQ; Tallis et al., 1992). An instrument that is gaining popularity in the measurement of obsessive–compulsive (O–C) symptoms is the Padua Inventory (PI; Sanavio, 1988). However, it has been noted that measures of worry and obsessions present a number of limitations to researchers wishing to distinguish between worry and obsessions and wishing to explore the differences between these types of ideational events (e.g. Freeston et al., 1994; Burns et al., 1996). To facilitate research, self-report instruments that reliably distinguish between worry and obsessions are required. The nature of the common variance in existing measures should be determined for purposes of refinement of psychometric measurement and for the development of theories of worry and obsessions.

Worry and obsessions share a number of features (Turner et al., 1992; Wells and Morrison, 1994; Clark and Claybourn, 1997). However, there are a number of differences. In a comparison of normal worries and normal obsessions, Wells and Morrison (1994)demonstrated that worries tended to be more verbal, realistic and voluntary and were associated with ratings of a greater compulsion to act than obsessions. Despite these differences, considerable similarity existed. For example, worry and obsessions did not differ in intrusiveness, controllability, dismissability, the extent to which they were resisted and how distracting or distressing the thoughts were. A possible difference between worry and obsessions which remains to be explored concerns the functional significance of these types of thoughts. Worry has been viewed as avoidance (Borkovec and Inz, 1991), as a form of problem-solving (Davey, 1994) and as a coping activity (Wells, 1994a, Wells, 1995), whilst no functional significance appears to have been assigned to obsessions.

One of the differentiating characteristics between worry and obsessions that has been used to improve the distinguishability of these phenomena is thought content (APA, 1994; Burns et al., 1996). In particular, worries are considered to consist of apprehensive expectation about real-life events such as finances, relations and work (APA, 1994). According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; APA, 1994, p. 435), ``Several features distinguish the excessive worry of Generalized Anxiety Disorder from the obsessional thoughts of Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. Obsessional thoughts are not simply excessive worries about everyday or real-life problems, but rather are ego-dystonic intrusions that often take the form of urges, impulses and images in addition to thoughts''. In summary, worry and obsessions are currently distinguished on the basis of content. The content of obsessions is considered to be ego-dystonic, that is to say, it is viewed as alien or inappropriate by the individual. Typical obsessions concern sexual, aggressive and contamination themes that are out of character. However, the content distinction is not free from difficulties. In particular, some obsessional symptoms such as contamination fears are not necessarily ego-dystonic and are likely to overlap with worry themes such as those concerning health. Moreover, a distinction of obsessional phenomena purely in terms of their ego-dystonicity could lead to the neglect of other idiosyncratic features that could reliably differentiate obsessions from worry.

On the basis of possible content differences, Burns et al. (1996)set out to revise the Padua Inventory (PI) and reduce its overlap with worry. A problem with the initial PI was that items could be answered with reference to either worry or obsessions (e.g. ``when doubts and worries come to my mind, I cannot rest until I have talked them over with a reassuring person''). Most of these problematic items were on the `impaired control over mental activities' subscale of the PI. Burns et al. (1996)attempted to eliminate this problem by selecting 39 of the original 60 items of the PI and organising them into content categories relevant to obsessions and compulsions. However, the content of items retained in the `obsessional thoughts about harm to self/others' category may remain equally applicable to worry and obsessions (e.g. ``I think or worry at length about having hurt someone without knowing it''). Therefore, difficulties are likely in the independence of this category from measures of worry. For example, `worry at length about having hurt someone' could refer to social-evaluative worry or to obsessions. In order to test the discriminant validity of the revised PI (Padua Inventory-Washington State University Revision or PI-WSUR), Burns et al. (1996)computed correlations for the modified subscales with the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer et al., 1990). However, the PSWQ is not considered to be a content-based measure of worry but assesses general worry frequency and distress. Questions concerning the degree of overlap between the PI-WSUR and a content-based measure of worry remain to be addressed.

The present study aimed to further explore the relationship between worry and O–C symptoms. We examined correlations between the PI-WSUR and the PSWQ and used the Anxious Thoughts Inventory (AnTI) to explore further the distinctiveness of the PI-WSUR. The AnTI is a multi-dimensional measure of worry assessing dimensions of health, social and meta-worry (worry about worry). For purposes of this study, only the social and health worry subscales were analyzed. In addition to exploring the overlap between the PI-WSUR and the AnTI, we tested relationships between worry, O–C symptoms and beliefs. Beliefs about one's own thoughts, or meta-cognitive beliefs, have been linked to vulnerability to emotional disorder (Wells and Matthews, 1994, Wells, 1997Wells and Matthews, 1996a). In particular, Wells (1995), Wells (1997)has advanced a cognitive model of GAD in which positive and negative beliefs about worry are central to problem maintenance. Meta-cognitive beliefs have also been linked to OCD. For example, Clark and Purdon, 1993, Clark and Purdon, 1995suggest that obsessional individuals hold dysfunctional beliefs concerning the need to control thoughts. In a related vein, it has been suggested that an inflated sense of responsibility plays a significant role in OCD (Rachman, 1976Rachman, 1993; Salkovskis, 1985). Wells (1997)suggests that responsibility appraisals are an emergent property of meta-cognitive beliefs about the power and consequence of obsessional thoughts. In an analysis of the structure of responsibility, Rachman et al. (1995)demonstrated that the Thought Action Fusion (TAF) subscale of the Responsibility Appraisal Questionnaire (RAQ) was particularly significant to O–C symptoms. TAF is the belief that thoughts can influence events or are almost equivalent to actions. Given the importance of meta-cognitive beliefs, we tested for relationships between a measure of pathological worry (PSWQ) and meta-cognitive beliefs. Relationships between meta-cognitive beliefs and O–C symptoms were also investigated. In doing so, we controlled for the overlap between worry and O–C symptoms.

In this study, we tested the following specific hypotheses: (1) a content-based worry measure (AnTI) will show significant overlap with the PI-WSUR. (2) Meta-cognitive beliefs about worry and intrusions will be associated with a measure of pathological worry (PSWQ). More specifically, in accordance with Wells' (Wells, 1995, Wells, 1997) cognitive model of GAD, positive and negative beliefs about worry will be positively associated with pathological worry. (3) Meta-cognitive beliefs about worry and intrusions will be associated with O–C symptoms even when the overlap with worry is controlled. (4) Since compulsive checking may be viewed as a way of preventing future danger (e.g. Tallis and de Silva, 1992), we predict that it has a similar function to worry and may be conceptualised as a behavioural variant of worry. Therefore, we hypothesised that compulsive checking would be positively associated with positive beliefs about worry. However, contamination and washing compulsions serve a somewhat different function, since in many instances the individual believes that they have already encountered the danger (contaminant). Therefore, we hypothesised that there would be a different pattern of meta-cognitive predictors of washing compulsions. We did not have more detailed a priori hypotheses concerning the precise nature of predictors in this case, or of the predictors of PI-WSUR impulses and dressing compulsions.

Section snippets

Measures

To explore the relationships between worry, O–C symptoms and meta-cognitive beliefs, the following measures were used:

(1) Anxious Thoughts Inventory (AnTI; Wells, 1994b): the AnTI is a 22-item self-report content and process measure of proneness to three dimensions of worry: health worry (e.g. ``I worry about having a heart attack or cancer''), social worry (e.g. ``I worry about making a fool of myself'') and meta-worry (e.g. ``I worry that I cannot control my thoughts as well as I would like

Results

Apart from scores on the PSWQ and the AnTI social worry subscale, all other scores were skewed. A log transformation was performed on all of the skewed variables and this was successful in `normalising' their distributions. Since some subjects scored zero on a number of subscales of the PI-WSUR, a numerical constant of 1 was added to all scores on these particular subscales prior to performing log transformations. The anti-logged means (minus 1 where appropriate) and 95% confidence limits for

Discussion

The present study set out to test the independence of measures of worry and O–C symptoms and to explore the meta-cognitive predictors of these variables. The results of correlational analyses suggested that the revised Padua Inventory is an improvement over the original scale in its reduced overlap with worry as measured by the PSWQ. However, we found that the revised scale continues to show significant overlap with both content and non-content worry measures.

The overlap between the revised

References (35)

  • S. Rachman

    Obsessional–compulsive checking

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1976)
  • S. Rachman

    Obsessions, responsibility and guilt

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1993)
  • S. Rachman et al.

    Abnormal and normal obsessions

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1978)
  • S. Rachman et al.

    Perceived responsibility: structure and significance

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1995)
  • P.M. Salkovskis

    Obsessional–compulsive problems: a cognitive-behavioural analysis

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1985)
  • P.M. Salkovskis et al.

    Abnormal and normal obsessions: a replication

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1984)
  • E. Sanavio

    Obsessions and compulsions: the Padua Inventory

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1988)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text